Sunae Kim
Senior Lecturer in Psychology
School of Psychology, Social Work and Public Health
Teaching and supervision
Modules taught
PSYC4011. Contemporary issues in Psychology
PSYC5020. Attachment & Human Development
PSYC6007. Topics in Developmental Psychology
PSYC6011. Final year Psychology Project
PSYC7007. Research Dissertation
PSYC7008. Cognitive and Social Aspects of Development
Research
I am interested in a broad range of topics in social cognition, particularly in relation to children’s socially guided learning, metacognition and mindreading/theory of mind.
Groups
Publications
Journal articles
-
Kim S, Arif MI, 'Children’s expectations of selective informing: The role of informational relevance on group membership based informing'
Cognitive Development 71 (2024)
ISSN: 0885-2014 eISSN: 1879-226XAbstractPublished hereSurprisingly little is known about how informational relevance guides children’s informing decisions. Although prior studies have demonstrated that children selectively inform and teach others these studies do not directly address whether children consider informational relevance specific to an outgroup member. We also know that children by age 5 and 6 show robust preferences for their ingroup members in various decisions but does information relevance modulate their ingroup preferences? In three experiments (N = 180), we investigated whether Iraqi Kurdish 6-year-old children expect others to inform an ingroup member or an outgroup member, depending on the informational relevance. In Experiment 1 children expected others to inform an ingroup member rather than an outgroup member irrespective of information type – extending prior work on ingroup preferences. In experiments 2 and 3, in which the relevance of the information to an outgroup member was highlighted, children’s expectation about informing an ingroup member was modulated by information type. Together, the findings suggest that children consider informational relevance to guide their expectations about others’ selective informing in the context of group membership, which could further explain how cultural knowledge is maintained and reinforced among members of the same cultural group.
-
Kim S, Sakaki M, Murayama K , 'Metacognition of curiosity: People underestimate the seductive lure of non-instrumental information'
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 31 (3) (2024) pp.1233-1244
ISSN: 1069-9384 eISSN: 1531-5320AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARCuriosity – the desire to seek information – is fundamental for learning and performance. Studies on curiosity have shown that people are intrinsically motivated to seek information even if it does not bring an immediate tangible benefit (i.e., non-instrumental information), but little is known as to whether people have the metacognitive capability to accurately monitor their motivation for seeking information. We examined whether people can accurately predict their own non-instrumental information-seeking behavior. Across six experiments (Experiments 1A–1E and 2, total N = 579), participants predicted that they would engage in information-seeking behavior less frequently than they actually did, suggesting that people tend to underestimate the motivational lure of curiosity. Overall, there was no consistent statistical evidence that this underestimation was altered by contextual factors (e.g., the cost to seek information). These results were consistent with the theoretical account that it is difficult for people to make sense of the internally rewarding value of information in advance.
-
Kim S, Senju A, Sodian B, Paulus M, Itakura S, Okuno A, Ueno M, Proust J, 'Memory Monitoring and Control in Japanese and German Preschoolers'
Memory & Cognition 51 (3) (2023) pp.708-717
ISSN: 0090-502X eISSN: 1532-5946AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrior studies explored the early development of memory monitoring and control. However, little work has examined cross-cultural similarities and differences in metacognitive development in early childhood. In the present research, we investigated a total of 100 Japanese and German preschool-aged children’s memory monitoring and control in a visual perception task. After seeing picture items, some of which were repeated, children were presented with picture pairs, one of which had been presented earlier and the other was a novel item. They then were asked to identify the previously presented picture. Children were also asked to evaluate their confidence about their selection, and to sort the responses to be used for being awarded with a prize at the end of the test. Both groups similarly expressed more confidence in the accurately remembered items than in the inaccurately remembered items, and their sorting decision was based on their subjective confidence. Japanese children’s sorting more closely corresponded to memory accuracy than German children’s sorting, however. These findings were further confirmed by a hierarchical Bayesian estimation of metacognitive efficiency. The present findings therefore suggest that early memory monitoring and control have both culturally similar and diverse aspects. The findings are discussed in light of broader sociocultural influences on metacognition.
-
Kim S, Le Guen O, Sodian B, Proust J, 'Are Children Sensitive to What They Know?: An Insight from Yucatec Mayan Children'
Journal of Cognition and Culture 21 (3/4) (2021) pp.226-242
ISSN: 1567-7095 eISSN: 1568-5373AbstractPublished hereMetacognitive abilities are considered as a hallmark of advanced human cognition. Existing empirical studies have exclusively focused on populations from Western and industrialized societies. Little is known about young children’s metacognitive abilities in other societal and cultural contexts. Here we tested 4-year-old Yucatec Mayan (a rural native population from Mexico) by adopting a metacognitive task in which children’s explicit assessment of their own knowledge states about the hidden content of a container and their informing judgments (whether or not to inform an ignorant person about the hidden contents of a container) were assessed. Similar to previous studies, we found that Yucatec Mayan children overestimated their knowledge states in the explicit metacognitive task. However, in contrast with studies on Western children, we did not find the facilitating effect of the implicit informing task over the explicit task. These findings suggest that the early development of metacognition combines universal and culture-sensitive features.
-
Kim S, Kristen-Antonow S, Sodian B, 'A longitudinal study of early pretense: Metarepresentational or not'
International Journal of Behavioral Development 45 (4) (2021) pp.345-354
ISSN: 0165-0254 eISSN: 1464-0651AbstractPublished hereThe metarepresentational aspect of early pretend play (make-believe activities where children create or participate in creating a new situation different from a real one) has been theoretically debated. In the present longitudinal study of N = 83 children, we tested for predictive relations of shared attention at 12–18 months, implicit false belief (FB) at 18 months, and pretend production at 18 months, as well as comprehension at 24 months. We also tested for long-term predictive relations of pretense production and comprehension with theory of mind (ToM) at the age of 4–5 years. Only pretense production directed toward others (but not self) was specifically related to infancy measures of shared attention. Early pretense, either production or comprehension, was not related to implicit FB or later ToM measures. The findings are discussed in terms of different theoretical accounts of early pretense.
-
Kloo D, Sodian B, Kristen‐Antonow S, Kim S, Paulus M, 'Knowing minds: Linking early perspective taking and later metacognitive insight'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 39 (1) (2020) pp.39-53
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835XAbstractPublished hereRecent metacognitive research using a partial knowledge task indicates that a firm understanding of ‘knowing about knowing’ develops surprisingly late, at around 6 years of age. To reveal the mechanisms subserving this development, the partial knowledge task was used in a longitudinal study with 67 children (33 girls) as an outcome measure at 5;9 (years;months). In addition, first- and second-order false belief was assessed at 4;2, 5;0, and 5;9. At 2;6, perspective taking and executive abilities were evaluated. Metacognition at 5;9 was correlated with earlier theory of mind and perspective taking – even when verbal intelligence and executive abilities were partialled out. This highlights the importance of perspective taking for the development of an understanding of one’s own mind.
-
Kim S, Sodian B, Paulus M, Senju A, Okuno A, Ueno M, Itakura S, Proust J, 'Metacognition and mindreading in young children: A cross-cultural study'
Consciousness and Cognition 85 (2020)
ISSN: 1053-8100 eISSN: 1090-2376AbstractPublished herePrior studies document cross cultural variation in the developmental onset of mindreading. In particular, Japanese children are reported to pass a standard false belief task later than children from Western countries. By contrast, we know little about cross-cultural variation in young children’s metacognitive abilities. Moreover, one prominent theoretical discussion in developmental psychology focuses on the relation between metacognition and mindreading. Here we investigated the relation between mindreading and metacognition (both implicit and explicit) by testing 4-year-old Japanese and German children. We found no difference in metacognition between the two cultural groups. By contrast, Japanese children showed lower performance than German children replicating cultural differences in mindreading. Finally, metacognition and mindreading were not related in either group. We discuss the findings in light of the existing theoretical accounts of the relation between metacognition and mindreading.
-
Kim S, Sodian B, Proust J, '12- and 24-Month-Old Infants’ Search Behavior Under Informational Uncertainty'
Frontiers in Psychology 11 (2020)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished hereInfants register and react to informational uncertainty in the environment. They also form expectations about the probability of future events as well as update the expectation according to changes in the environment. A novel line of research has started to investigate infants’ and toddlers’ behavior under uncertainty. By combining these research areas, the present research investigated 12- and 24-month-old infants’ searching behaviors under varying degree of informational uncertainty. An object was hidden in one of three possible locations and probabilistic information about the hiding location was manipulated across trials. Infants’ time delay in search initiation for a hidden object linearly increased across the level of informational uncertainty. Infants’ successful searching also varied according to probabilistic information. The findings suggest that infants modulate their behaviors based on probabilistic information. We discuss the possibility that infants’ behavioral reaction to the environmental uncertainty constitutes the basis for the development of subjective uncertainty.
-
Kim S, Paulus M, Sodian B, Proust J, 'Children’s prior experiences of their successes and failures modulate belief alignment'
European Journal of Developmental Psychology 17 (5) (2020) pp.664-678
ISSN: 1740-5629 eISSN: 1740-5610AbstractPublished hereAn ability to flexibly learn from others while at other times relying upon one’s own judgements is an important adaptive human capacity. The present research investigated how others’ epistemic states and prior experience of their own independent ability in a given task modulate young children’s selective learning. In particular, we asked whether 4-year-old children’s judgement concerning the location of a hidden object is modulated both by an informant’s knowledge states and by the absence/presence of a prior experience with a particular task. We found that the children were more likely to align their judgement according to the informant’s verbal report when the informant was knowledgeable than when she was ignorant – but only when they had explicitly experienced their own incapability to accurately guess an object’s location. The findings suggest that 4-year-old children are able to combine their own experience with others’ input to make their judgement.
-
Kim S, Spelke ES, 'Learning from multiple informants: Children’s response to epistemic bases for consensus judgments'
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 192 (2020)
ISSN: 0022-0965 eISSN: 1096-0457AbstractPublished hereConsensus has both social and epistemic value. Children conform to consensus judgments in ways that suggest they are sensitive to the social value of consensus. Here we report two experiments providing evidence that 4-year-old children also are sensitive to the epistemic value of consensus. When multiple informants gave the same judgment concerning the hidden contents of a container, based on the observation of one of their members, children’s own judgments tended to align with the consensus judgment over the judgment of a lone character, whose observation received no endorsements. This tendency was reduced, however, when children were shown that the group consensus lacked epistemic warrant. Together, the findings provide evidence that young children are sensitive to the epistemic basis of consensus reports.
-
Kim S, Paulus M, Sodian B, Itakura S, Ueno M, Senju A, Proust J, 'Selective learning and teaching among Japanese and German children.'
Developmental Psychology 54 (3) (2018) pp.536-542
ISSN: 0012-1649 eISSN: 1939-0599AbstractPublished hereDespite an increasing number of studies demonstrating that young children selectively learn from others, and a few studies of children’s selective teaching, the evidence almost exclusively comes from Western cultures, and cross-cultural comparison in this line of work is very rare. In the present research, we investigated Japanese and German children’s selective learning and teaching abilities. We found clear cultural differences. Japanese children were better at selectively teaching an ignorant person over a knowledgeable person than at selectively learning from knowledgeable others. By contrast, German children were better at choosing to learn from a knowledgeable rather than from an ignorant person than at selectively teaching ignorant others. The present findings suggest that the development of human learning and teaching, especially the tendency to take into account others’ knowledge status, is strongly affected by cultural background.
-
Kim S, Paulus M, Kalish C, 'Young Children's Reliance on Information From Inaccurate Informants'
Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal 41 (S3) (2016) pp.601-621
ISSN: 0364-0213 eISSN: 1551-6709AbstractPublished herePrior work shows that children selectively learn from credible speakers. Yet little is known how they treat information from non-credible speakers. This research examined to what extent and under what conditions children may or may not learn from problematic sources. In three studies, we found that children displayed trust toward previously inaccurate speakers. Children were equally likely to extend labels from previously accurate and inaccurate speakers to novel objects. Moreover, they expected third parties to share labels provided by previously inaccurate speakers. Only when there was clear evidence that the speakers' information was wrong (as in the case when speakers' perceptual access to the information was blocked), did young children reject the label. Together, the findings provide evidence that young children do not completely ignore the labels supplied by non-credible speakers unless there is strong reason to do so.
-
Kim S, Paulus M, Sodian B, Proust J, 'Young Children’s Sensitivity to Their Own Ignorance in Informing Others'
PLoS ONE 11 (3) (2016)
ISSN: 1932-6203 eISSN: 1932-6203AbstractPublished herePrior research suggests that young children selectively inform others depending on others’ knowledge states. Yet, little is known whether children selectively inform others depending on their own knowledge states. To explore this issue, we manipulated 3- to 4-year-old children’s knowledge about the content of a box and assessed the impact on their decisions to inform another person. Moreover, we assessed the presence of uncertainty gestures while they inform another person in light of the suggestions that children's gestures reflect early developing, perhaps transient, epistemic sensitivity. Finally, we compared children’s performance in the informing context to their explicit verbal judgment of their knowledge states to further confirm the existence of a performance gap between the two tasks. In their decisions to inform, children tend to accurately assess their ignorance, whereas they tend to overestimate their own knowledge states when asked to explicitly report them. Moreover, children display different levels of uncertainty gestures depending on the varying degrees of their informational access. These findings suggest that children’s implicit awareness of their own ignorance may be facilitated in a social, communicative context.
-
Kim S, Kalish CW., Weisman K, Johnson MV., Shutts K, 'Young Children Choose to Inform Previously Knowledgeable Others'
Journal of Cognition and Development 17 (2) (2015) pp.320-340
ISSN: 1524-8372 eISSN: 1532-7647AbstractPublished hereChildren recognize that people who know more are better informants than those who know less. How does an individual’s prior knowledge affect children’s decisions about whom to inform? In 3 experiments, 3- to 6-year-old children were invited to share a novel piece of information with 1 of 2 potential recipients who differed in their recent history of knowledge. Children tended to inform the previously knowledgeable person rather than the previously ignorant person. This same effect was observed in a 4th experiment when the knowledgeable person stated that she already knew the information the participant had to share. In no case was the opposite pattern observed: Children never chose to inform the person who had known less. These results seem to conflict with equity considerations and may reflect a preference to affiliate with competent social partners.
-
Paulus M, Kim S, Sodian B, 'Clarifying the range of social-cognitive processes subserving human teaching'
Behavioral and Brain Sciences: An International Journal of Current Research and Theory with Open Peer Commentary 38 (2015)
ISSN: 0140-525X eISSN: 1469-1825AbstractPublished hereAn evolutionary framework on human teaching is not well equipped to explain the nature of human teaching unless it specifies the subserving cognitive and motivational mechanisms. Only a theory that speculates on the psychological processes provides testable predictions and stimulates further empirical research.
-
Kim S, Harris PL, 'Selecting among extraordinary informants'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 32 (4) (2014) pp.394-396
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835XAbstractPublished hereIn our reply to the commentaries by Jacqui Woolley and Rebekah Richert, we discuss the following: (1) possible effects that religious educations have on individual differences and (2) how older children may construe mind-readers.
-
Kim S, Sodian B, Paulus M, 'Does he need help or can he help himself? Preschool children's expectations about others' instrumental helping versus self-helping'
Frontiers in Psychology 5 (2014)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished hereIn the present study, we investigated a total of fifty-one 3.5-, 4.5-, and 5.5-year-old children’s expectations about another person’s helping behaviors. We asked children to complete a story in which one person failed to complete his goal (e.g., because an object was misplaced or put out of his reach) while the other person observed the event. We asked whether the children expected the other person to help the protagonist or whether they expected the protagonist to help himself. Children of 3.5 years expected the other person to provide help in the majority of trials. In contrast, the older children were equally likely to predict that the other person would help the protagonist or the protagonist would help himself.
-
Kim S, Harris PL, 'Children prefer to learn from mind‐readers'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 32 (4) (2014) pp.375-387
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835XAbstractPublished hereKim and Harris (2014, J. Cogn. Dev.) showed that children selectively learned from an informant who produced apparently magical outcomes as compared to another informant who produced only ordinary outcomes in the domain of everyday physics. In the present study, we tested children's ability to differentiate between and selectively learn from informants who displayed either an extraordinary or an ordinary ability in the domain of everyday psychology. Prior studies have shown that children come to appreciate what is ordinarily involved in knowing the private mental states of other people. Drawing on this research, we asked whether 3- to 4- and 5- to 6-year-old children preferentially learned from an informant who knew another person's mind via either an ordinary or an extraordinary form of communication. As compared to younger children, older children were more likely to learn from the extraordinary informant. Moreover, children's ability to differentiate between the two informants was a better predictor of their learning from the extraordinary informant than age. We discuss the findings in the light of prior work on selective learning, children's ideas about prayer and their understanding of impossibility.
-
Kim S, Harris PL, Warneken F, 'Is it okay to tell? Children's judgements about information disclosure'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 32 (3) (2014) pp.291-304
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835XAbstractPublished hereThe present research investigates how young children evaluate and reason about the disclosure of private information. Using story vignettes, children aged 4–5 and 7–8 years were asked to evaluate an individual who passed on information from a peer revealing that he or she had broken a rule (e.g., stolen a cookie; rule type) or lacked a skill (e.g., could not ride a bicycle; competence type). These negative valence stories were compared with positive valence stories in which the peer had followed a rule or possessed a skill. Younger children approved the sharing of positive, but not negative, information, irrespective of type (rule vs. competence). Older children disapproved the disclosure of someone's incompetence, whereas they approved the disclosure of a rule violation. Children justified their evaluations by reference to social rules in the rule-type vignettes and to an individual's feelings in the competence-type vignettes. The findings suggest that young children are concerned about the disclosure of negative information about other people, but with age they become increasingly concerned about protecting the social order even at the cost of individual privacy.
-
Kim S, Harris PL, 'Belief in Magic Predicts Children's Selective Trust in Informants'
Journal of Cognition and Development 15 (2) (2014) pp.181-196
ISSN: 1524-8372 eISSN: 1532-7647AbstractPublished hereChildren are able to distinguish between regular events that can occur in everyday reality and magical events that are ordinarily impossible. How do children respond to a person who brings about magical as compared with ordinary outcomes? In two studies, we tested children's acceptance of informants' claims when the informants had produced either magical or ordinary outcomes. In Study 1, children's skeptical or credulous stance toward magic predicted their endorsement of the claims made by the informants. Children who were more credulous were likely to accept information from the informant who had produced magical outcomes. In Study 2, a brief manipulation was only partially effective in changing children's initial stance toward magic. Their initial stance toward magic continued to predict their acceptance of information from the informant who had produced magical outcomes.
-
Kalish CW, Kim S, Young AG, 'How Young Children Learn From Examples: Descriptive and Inferential Problems'
Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal 36 (8) (2012) pp.1427-1448
ISSN: 0364-0213 eISSN: 1551-6709AbstractPublished hereThree experiments with preschool- and young school-aged children (N = 75 and 53) explored the kinds of relations children detect in samples of instances (descriptive problem) and how they generalize those relations to new instances (inferential problem). Each experiment initially presented a perfect biconditional relation between two features (e.g., all and only frogs are blue). Additional examples undermined one of the component conditional relations (not all frogs are blue) but supported another (only frogs are blue). Preschool-aged children did not distinguish between supported and undermined relations. Older children did show the distinction, at least when the test instances were clearly drawn from the same population as the training instances. Results suggest that younger children’s difficulties may stem from the demands of using imperfect correlations for predictions. Older children seemed sensitive to the inferential problem of using samples to make predictions about populations.
-
Kim S, Kalish CW, Harris PL, 'Speaker reliability guides children's inductive inferences about novel properties'
Cognitive Development 27 (2) (2012) pp.114-125
ISSN: 0885-2014 eISSN: 1879-226XAbstractPublished herePrior work shows that children can make inductive inferences about objects based on their labels rather than their appearance (Gelman, 2003). A separate line of research shows that children's trust in a speaker's label is selective. Children accept labels from a reliable speaker over an unreliable speaker (e.g., Koenig & Harris, 2005). In the current paper, we tested whether 3- and 5-year-old children attend to speaker reliability when they make inductive inferences about a non-obvious property of a novel artifact based on its label. Children were more likely to use a reliable speaker's label than an unreliable speaker's label when making inductive inferences. Thus, children not only prefer to learn from reliable speakers, they are also more likely to use information from reliable speakers as the basis for future inferences. The findings are discussed in light of the debate between a similarity-driven and a label-driven approach to inductive inferences.
-
Kim S, Kalish CW, 'Children's ascriptions of property rights with changes of ownership'
Cognitive Development 24 (3) (2009) pp.322-336
ISSN: 0885-2014 eISSN: 1879-226XAbstractPublished hereOwnership is not a “natural” property of objects, but is determined by human intentions. Facts about who owns what may be altered by appropriate decisions. However, young children often deny the efficacy of transfer decisions, asserting that original owners retain rights to their property. In Experiment 1, 4–5-year-old and 7–8-year-old children and adults were asked to resolve disputes between initial owners and various types of receivers (finders, borrowers, buyers). Experiment 2 involved disputes both before and after transfers of ownership. At all ages participants privileged owners over non-owners and accepted the effectiveness of property transfers. Overall, children's intuitions about property rights were similar to those of adults. Observed differences may reflect older participants’ willingness to segregate property rights from other considerations in assessing the acceptability of actions.
-
Nathan MJ, Kim S, 'Pattern Generalization with Graphs and Words: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analysis of Middle School Students' Representational Fluency'
Mathematical Thinking and Learning 9 (3) (2007) pp.193-219
ISSN: 1098-6065 eISSN: 1532-7833AbstractPublished hereCross-sectional and longitudinal data from students as they advance through the middle school years (grades 6–8) reveal insights into the development of students' pattern generalization abilities. As expected, students show a preference for lower-level tasks such as reading the data, over more distant predictions and generation of abstractions. Performance data also indicate a verbal advantage that shows greater success when working with words than graphs, a replication of earlier findings comparing words to symbolic equations. Surprisingly, students show a marked advantage with patterns presented in a continuous format (line graphs and verbal rules) as compared to those presented as collections of discrete instances (point-wise graphs and lists of exemplars). Student pattern–generalization performance also was higher when words and graphs were combined. Analyses of student performance patterns and strategy use contribute to an emerging developmental model of representational fluency. The model contributes to research on the development of representational fluency and can inform instructional practices and curriculum design in the area of algebraic development. Results also underscore the impact that perceptual aspects of representations have on students' reasoning, as suggested by an Embodied Cognition view.
Books
-
Harris M, Westermann G, Kim S, Bazhydai M, A Student's Guide to Developmental Psychology, Routledge Taylor and Francis group (2024)
ISBN: 9780367471040AbstractPublished hereNow in its second edition, this fundamental undergraduate textbook provides students with everything they need when studying developmental psychology.
Thoroughly revised, this book breaks down key topics into easily accessible concepts and provides students with both an overview of traditional research and theory as well as an insight into the latest research findings and techniques. Taking a chronological approach, the key milestones from birth to adolescence are highlighted and clear links between changes in behaviour and developments in brain activity are made. A new chapter provides a global perspective on development, including findings regarding children’s motor, cognitive, literacy, social and emotional development, as well as the importance of cross-cultural studies and their challenges. Each chapter also highlights both typical and atypical developments, as well as discussing and contrasting the effects of genetic and environmental factors.
This textbook comes with a wealth of carefully updated pedagogical features, designed to help students engage with the material, including:
• Learning objectives for every chapter
• Key term definitions
• Over 100 colour illustrations
• Chapter summaries
• Further reading
• Suggested essay questions.A Student’s Guide to Developmental Psychology is accompanied by a support material package, featuring a range of helpful supplementary resources including exclusive video clips to illustrate key developmental concepts, multiple-choice questions, flashcards and more.
This book is essential reading for all undergraduate students of developmental psychology. It will also be of interest to those in education, healthcare and other subjects requiring an up-to-date and accessible overview of child development. -- Provided by publisher.
Book chapters
-
Kim S, Shahaeian A, Proust J, 'Developmental diversity in mindreading and metacognition' in Metacognitive Diversity: An Interdisciplinary Approach, Oxford University Press (2018)
ISBN: 9780198789710 eISBN: 9780191841675AbstractPublished hereA first aim of this chapter is to explain why children seem to present different patterns of development across cultures for solving false-belief tasks. Anthropological evidence is presented suggesting that the tests devised for Western children might not be adequate outside Western cultures. Alternative practices and values, such as the willingness/refusal to express one’s own mental states, the degree of autonomous agency allocated to young children, and the style of communication used in child-rearing, might partly explain the timing differences in the development of mindreading. A second aim is to identify the sociocultural factors that might also differentially impact the development of metacognitive abilities. It is proposed that the cultural practices that regulate patterns of attention, ways of learning, and communicational pragmatics should differentially influence the kinds of epistemic decisions that need to be monitored and the process of attribution of knowledge to the self in young children.
-
Fortier M, Kim S, 'From the Impossible to the Improbable: A Probabilistic Account of Magical Beliefs and Practices Across Development and Cultures' in The Science of Lay Theories, Springer International Publishing (2017)
ISBN: 9783319573052 eISBN: 9783319573069AbstractPublished hereThis chapter discusses the cognitive mechanisms underlying magical beliefs and practices. We first review empirical studies in developmental psychology that address children’s concepts of magic. In particular, these studies focus on how children come to distinguish between events, entities, and agents that violate our intuitive notions of basic causal laws (e.g., gravity) and those that do not. The second part of the chapter reviews anthropological studies on magic (i.e., on witchcraft, shamanism, animism, etc.). The striking feature of these anthropological data is that adults seem to interpret improbable rather than impossible events as magical. From this anthropological evidence, we then suggest that the current theoretical accounts of magic in developmental psychology and cognitive science of religion fail to tackle the pervasiveness of “probabilistic-magic”; as a result, it remains to be elucidated why people resort to magical explanations when faced with merely improbable events. To this end, we propose a new probabilistic account of magic, which predicts that supernatural explanations are triggered every time a complexity drop (i.e., a gap between expected and observed complexity) occurs. Finally, we address the question of knowing how “counterintuitive-magic” and “probabilistic-magic” are respectively instantiated across development and cultures.