Professor Margaret Harris
BSc PhD FBPsS
Emeritus Professor of Psychology
School of Psychology, Social Work and Public Health
Role
Margaret Harris began her career as a lecturer at Birkbeck College, moving to Royal Holloway University of London in 1989 as a Senior Lecturer and then Reader and Professor. In 2006 she moved to Oxford Brookes University to become Head of the Psychology Department and Director of the new Institute for Research in Child Development. In 2012 the Department of Psychology merged with the Department of Social Work and Public Health. In 2016, Margaret retired as Head of Department and is now focusing on her research in a part-time role.
Research
Margaret's main interests are in the development of written and spoken language and she has published widely in this area. Much of her research in the past two decades has focused on the language development of deaf children, especially the dynamics of deaf and hearing mothers' communication with their deaf children and literacy attainment in deaf school children and young people. She is the author of five books, and has recently published a new undergraduate textbook on developmental psychology. She was Editor of the British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2008-13), an Associate Editor of the Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education and a founding Editor of Mind & Language.
Research grants and awards
- ESRC 2013 - 2016 The impact of new technologies on the reading and phonological skills of deaf primary school children £332,181
- Technology Strategy Board 2010 –2012 National trial of Ultra Low Carbon Vehicles (with Mark Burgess, OBU)
£640,075 - Technology Strategy Board 2009 -2011 User perceptions of MINI E
(joint project with School of Technology at Oxford Brookes) £147,755 - British Academy 2009-2010 Comparing measures of mind-mindedness and parental reflective function (with Morag MacLean, OBU)£7,495
- Burdett Trust for Nursing 2009-2010 A pilot study to evaluate the accuracy of health visitor assessments of the quality of mother-infant interactions (with Jane Appleton, OBU and John Oates, Open University)£40,489
- ESRC 2009-2010 Reading attainment and reading strategies in deaf adolescents with a cochlear implant £80,272
Groups
Publications
Journal articles
-
Buchanan-Worster E, MacSweeney M, Pimperton H, Kyle F, Harris M, Beedie I, Ralph-Lewis A, Hulme C
, 'Speechreading Ability is Related to Phonological Awareness and Single-Word Reading in Both Deaf and Hearing Children'
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63 (11) (2021) pp.3775-3785
ISSN: 1092-4388 eISSN: 1558-9102AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPurpose. Speechreading (lipreading) is a correlate of reading ability in both deaf and hearing children. We investigated whether the relationship between speechreading and single-word reading is mediated by phonological awareness in deaf and hearing children.
Method. In two separate studies, 66 deaf children and 138 hearing children, aged 5–8 years old, were assessed on measures of speechreading, phonological awareness, and single-word reading. We assessed the concurrent relationships between latent variables measuring speechreading, phonological awareness, and single-word reading.
Results. In both deaf and hearing children, there was a strong relationship between speechreading and single-word reading, which was fully mediated by phonological awareness.
Conclusions. These results are consistent with ideas from previous studies that visual speech information contributes to the development of phonological representations in both deaf and hearing children, which, in turn, support learning to read. Future longitudinal and training studies are required to establish whether these relationships reflect causal effects.
-
Gonzalez-Gomez N, O’Brien F, Harris M, 'The effects of Prematurity and Socioeconomic deprivation on early Phonological Development: A Story of Two Different Delays'
Developmental Science 24 (2) (2020)
ISSN: 1363-755X eISSN: 1467-7687AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThere is evidence showing that both maturational and environmental factors can impact on later language development. On the one hand, preterm birth has been found to increase the risk of deficits in the preschool and school years. Preterm children show poorer auditory discrimination, reading difficulties, poor vocabulary, less complex expressive language and lower receptive understanding than their matched controls. On the other hand, socioeconomic status indicators (i.e. income, education, occupation) have been found to be strongly related to linguistic abilities during the preschool and school years. However, there is very little information about how these factors result in lower linguistic abilities. The present study addresses this issue. To do so, we investigated early phonological development in full and preterm infants from families classed as high or low SES. 76 infants were followed longitudinally at 7.5, 9, 10.5 and 12 months of age. At each test point, three studies explored infants’ phonetic, prosodic and phonotactic development, respectively. Results showed no significant differences between the phonetic or the phonotactic development of the preterm and the full-term infants. However, a time-lag between preterm and full-term developmental timing for prosody was found. Socioeconomic status did not have a significant effect on prosodic development. Nonetheless, phonetic and phonotactic development were affected by SES, infants from lower SES showed phonetic discrimination of non-native contrast and a preference for high-frequency sequences later than their more advantaged peers. Overall these results suggest that different constraints apply to the acquisition of different phonological subcomponents.
-
Pimperton H, Kyle F, Hulme C, Harris M, Beedie I, Ralph-Lewis A, Worster E, Rees R, Donlan C, MacSweeney M, 'Computerized speechreading training for deaf children: A randomised controlled trial'
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62 (8) (2019) pp.2882-2894
ISSN: 1092-4388 eISSN: 1558-9102AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPurpose: We developed and evaluated in a randomised controlled trial a computerised speechreading training programme to determine a) whether it is possible to train speechreading in deaf children and b) whether speechreading training results in improvements in phonological and reading skills. Previous studies indicate a relationship between speechreading and reading skill and further suggest this relationship may be mediated by improved phonological representations. This is important since many deaf children find learning to read to be very challenging. Method: Sixty-six deaf 5-7 year olds were randomised into speechreading and maths training arms. Each training programme was comprised of 10 minute sessions a day, 4 days a week for 12 weeks. Children were assessed on a battery of language and literacy measures before training, immediately after training, 3 months and 10 months after training. Results: We found no significant benefits for participants who completed the speechreading training, compared to those who completed the maths training, on the speechreading primary outcome measure. However, significantly greater gains were observed in the speechreading training group on one of the secondary measures of speechreading. There was also some evidence of beneficial effects of the speechreading training on phonological representations, however these effects were weaker. No benefits were seen to word reading. Conclusions: Speechreading skill is trainable in deaf children. However, to support early reading, training may need to be longer or embedded in a broader literacy programme. Nevertheless, a training tool that can improve speechreading is likely to be of great interest to professionals working with deaf children.
-
King N, Burgess M, Harris M, 'Electric vehicle drivers use better strategies to counter stereotype threat linked to pro-technology than to pro-environmental identities'
Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 60 (2018) pp.440-452
ISSN: 1369-8478 eISSN: 1873-5517AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBattery electric vehicles (BEVs) could play a crucial role in reducing greenhouse gas emissions around the world. Much research has examined the practical barriers to large-scale BEV uptake, but very little has examined the psychological barriers. The current study addresses this gap in the literature by investigating the effects of stereotype threat on BEV drivers. This psychological predicament occurs when an individual imagines or experiences being judged in terms of negative stereotypes about their social group. Qualitative thematic analysis of interview data revealed three distinct stereotypes that the BEV drivers imagined or reported other people to hold: eccentric, low-status environmentalists; hypocritical, high status environmentalists; and geeky technophiles. With regard to the first and second stereotypes, drivers tended to use individualist defence strategies by downplaying their proenvironmental attitudes and dissociating themselves from the undesirable environmentalist groups. With regard to the third stereotype, they tended to use more constructive, group-level defence strategies by perceiving their BEV driver ingroup as superior on the innovative technology adopter dimension compared to their non-BEV driver outgroup. Suggestions are made for countering the psychological barrier of stereotype threat, such as promoting images of BEV drivers as future-shaping market leaders and treating them as members of an influential and desirable consumer group.
-
Barlow-Brown F, Barker C, Harris M, 'Size and modality effects in Braille learning: Implications for the blind child from pre-reading sighted children'
British Journal of Educational Psychology 89 (1) (2018) pp.165-176
ISSN: 0007-0998 eISSN: 2044-8279AbstractBackground. Beginning readers are typically introduced to enlarged print and the size of this print decreases as readers become more fluent. In comparison beginning blind readers are expected to learn standard-sized Braille from the outset because past research suggests letter knowledge cannot be transferred across different sizes of Braille. Aims. The study aims to investigate whether learning Braille using an oversized pegboard, leads to faster, transferable, letter learning and whether performance is mediated by either tactile or visual learning. Sample. Sixty-eight children participated in the study. All children were sighted pre-readers with no previous knowledge of Braille. The children came from two nursery schools with an average age of 47.8 months. Methods. Children were taught specific Braille letters using either an enlarged pegboard or standard Braille. Two other groups of children were taught using visually presented Braille characters in either an enlarged or standard-sized and a further control group mirrored the experience of blind children in receiving non-specific tactile training prior to being introduced to Braille. In all tactile conditions it was ensured that the children did not visually experience any Braille for the duration of the study. Results. Results demonstrated that initially training children with large Braille tactually led to the best subsequent learning of standard Braille. Despite the fact that both initial visual and large tactual learning was significantly faster than learning standard Braille, when transferring letter knowledge to standard tactile Braille previous tactile experience with the large pegboard offered the most efficient route. Conclusions. Braille letter knowledge can be transferred across size and modality particularly effectively with large tactile Braille. This has significant implications for the education of blind children.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Harris M, Terlektsi, E, Kyle FE, 'Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading for deaf and hearing children in primary school'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 22 (2) (2017) pp.233-242
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractForty-one children with severe-profound prelingual hearing loss were assessed on single word reading, reading comprehension, English vocabulary, phonological awareness and speechreading at three time points, one year apart (T1, T2, T3). Their progress was compared with that of a group of hearing children of similar non-verbal IQ, initially reading at the same level. Single word reading improved at each assessment point for the deaf children but there was no growth in reading comprehension from T2 to T3. There were no differences between children with cochlear implants and those with hearing aids on either reading measure but orally-educated children had higher scores than children who signed in the classroom. English vocabulary and speechreading were the most consistent longitudinal predictors of reading for the deaf children. Phonological awareness was the most consistent longitudinal predictor for the hearing group and also a concurrent predictor of reading at T3 for both groups. There were many more significant correlations among the various measures for the deaf children than the hearing at both T1 and T3, suggesting that skills underpinning reading, including phonological awareness and vocabulary, are more closely related for deaf children. Implications of these findings for of deaf children’s literacy are explored.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Harris M, Terlektsi E, Kyle FE, 'Literacy outcomes for Primary School Children Who are Deaf and Hard of Hearing: A cohort comparison study'
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 60 (3) (2017) pp.701-711
ISSN: 1092-4388 eISSN: 1558-9102AbstractPurpose: This study compared the language and literacy of two cohorts of children with severe-profound hearing loss, recruited 10 years apart, to determine whether outcomes had improved in line with the introduction of newborn hearing screening and access to improved hearing aid technology.Published here Open Access on RADAR
Method: Forty-two deaf children, aged 5 -7 years with a mean unaided loss of 102 DB, were assessed on language, reading and phonological skills. Their performance was compared to that of a similar group of 32 deaf children assessed 10 years earlier, and also a group of 40 hearing children of similar single word reading ability.
Results: English vocabulary was significantly higher in the new cohort, although it was still below chronological age. Phonological awareness and reading ability had not significantly changed over time. In both cohorts English vocabulary predicted reading but phonological awareness was only a significant predictor for the new cohort.
Conclusions: The current results show that vocabulary knowledge of children with severe-profound hearing loss has improved over time but there has not been a commensurate improvement in phonological skills or reading. They suggest that children with severe-profound hearing loss will require continued support to develop robust phonological coding skills to underpin reading.
-
Harris M, 'The impact of new technologies on the literacy attainment of deaf children'
Topics in Language Disorders 35 (2) (2015) pp.120-132
ISSN: 0271-8294AbstractPublished hereTo become successful readers, hearing children require competence in both decoding—the ability to read individual words, underpinned by phonological skills and letter–sound knowledge—and linguistic comprehension—the ability to understand what they read—underpinned by language skills, including vocabulary knowledge. Children who are born with a severe–profound hearing loss, or who acquire such a loss in the first months of life, need to develop the same core skills in decoding and linguistic comprehension although they may develop these skills in a somewhat different manner from hearing peers. This review considers the impact on literacy of universal newborn hearing screening and of improvements in the technologies that give access to sound, including the provision of cochlear implants. The review shows that these new technologies have brought some notable improvements, especially in the early years at school, but that many children with severe–profound hearing loss still find reading a challenge and can benefit from continued support for literacy throughout their years at school.
-
Bunce L, Harris M, Burgess M, 'Charge Up Then Charge Out? Drivers' Perceptions and Experiences of Electric Vehicles in the UK.'
Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 59 (2014) pp.278-287
ISSN: 0965-8564AbstractPublished hereThe UK government has made substantial investments in electric transport as a potential means of reducing CO2 emissions (DoECC, 2012). This paper investigates responses to recharging plug-in battery electric vehicles from the perspective of electric vehicle (EV) drivers. Drivers in the UK Ultra Low Carbon Vehicle trial (n = 135, 29% female, M = 47 years) completed questionnaires and were interviewed to assess their attitudes and experiences before they obtained their EV and after driving the EV for 3 months. The results demonstrated that drivers were positive about recharging – preferring it to ‘refuelling’ – and they became more relaxed over time about the frequency of recharging. Drivers managed without using a public charging infrastructure although such an infrastructure may be desirable to promote EV use. Finally, there was an interesting difference in drivers’ awareness of the environmental impact of driving and recharging an EV before and after the trial in relation to CO2 emissions and the energy cycle. The results are discussed in relation to the implications for developing the future EV market.
-
Burgess M, Harris M, 'Electric vehicle drivers’ reported interactions with the public: driving stereotype change?'
Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 17 (February) (2013) pp.33-44
ISSN: 1369-8478 eISSN: 1873-5517AbstractImage and symbolic meanings of cars play a key role in their desirability and in consumers’ purchasing behaviour. Drivers in the United Kingdom Technology Strategy Board funded Ultra Low Carbon Vehicle trial were interviewed regarding their interactions with the general (non-EV driving) public. EV drivers’ accounts suggest that EVs are particularly susceptible to stereotyping, but that the stereotyping is in a state of flux. Three different categories of meaning emerged. ATraditional, affective-based negative stereotype exists, despite reflecting outdated associations with milk floats and older EV models. However, the current period of widespread EV trialling in the UK sees the emergence of additional EV stereotypes. Drivers encountered categories of meaning that wereAmbivalent: cognitively-based, ambivalence showed people to hold negative views and reservations but also to reveal a willingness to assess the current capabilities of EVs. Finally, drivers encountered people holdingPositivemeanings of EVs. Greater contact with drivers of contemporary EVs helped to develop these further. Based on a combination of affect, cognition and behaviour, it shows a segment of non-EV drivers to be engaged with the new technology and to see EVs as well developed now and also as cars of the future. We discuss the key factors underpinning each of the categories of meaning and indicate the ramifications for the likely future success of EV uptake.Published here -
Appleton J V, Harris M, Oates J, Kelly, C
, 'Evaluating health visitor assessments of mother-infant interactions: a mixed methods study'
International Journal of Nursing Studies 50 (1) (2013) pp.5-15
ISSN: 0020-7489AbstractSettingsPublished here
Two universities and two primary care trusts.
Participant
In Phase One 17 first-time mothers and their 6- to 16-week-old infants were recruited. In Phase Two, a sample of 12 health visitors participated.
Methods
The study incorporated two data collection phases. In Phase One, each mother's interaction with her baby was video-recorded for 20min in an observation laboratory. The video-recordings were coded and resultant data were analysed to derive a number of quantitative measures of interaction quality, including mothers' responsiveness and sensitivity to their infants as rated by the Global Ratings Scales (GRS) of Mother-Infant Interaction. In Phase Two, 12 health visitors rated and assessed 9 clips of the video-recorded mother-infant interactions. The rationales for their ratings were then explored through in-depth interviews. Health visitor ratings of the video clips were compared to the GRS ratings. The relationship between the main focus of each health visitor rating, as reported in the interview, and the consistency of ratings with the GRS ratings were then investigated.
Results
Correlations between individual health visitors' ratings and the GRS ratings ranged from .17 to .83 and were statistically significant in only four cases. There was a weak relationship with health visitors' years of experience (rs=.47, NS). When explaining their judgements, health visitors tended to comment on the mother's behaviours or the relationship between the mother and baby and often ignored the behaviour of the baby. There was a highly significant relationship between the consistency of health visitor/GRS ratings and the number of references to the baby in the health visitors' explanations (rs=.75, p=.005).
Conclusion
This study contributes to the understanding of how health visitors make assessments of mother-infant interactions. The frequent lack of attention and reference to the baby's behaviour suggests an area for further training. -
Bunce L, Harris M, 'He Hasn't Got the Real Toolkit! Young Children's Reasoning About Real/not-real Status'
Developmental Psychology 49 (8) (2013) pp.1494-1504
ISSN: 0012-1649 eISSN: 1939-0599AbstractDuring the preschool years, children develop an understanding of 2 types of real/not-real distinctions: ontological status and authenticity (Bunce & Harris, 2008). Two studies compared 3- to 5-year-old children’s real/not-real judgments and justifications for 3 types of contrast involving a real entity and either a fictional character, a child dressing-up, or a Lego toy. As expected, all children discriminated between the real entities and toys and dressing-up, and they justified their judgments on the basis of authenticity. In contrast, only older children consistently discriminated between the real entities and fictional characters on the basis of ontological status. In Study 2, the real and not-real entities were presented as pairs rather than individually to define the intended contrast. This manipulation increased children’s ability to discriminate between the real and not-real entities on the basis of authenticity. Together, these results support the hypothesis that understanding reality status on the basis of authenticity develops before ontological status. The development of reasoning about real/not-real status is discussed.Published here -
Harris M, 'Literacy in the Classroom and Beyond'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16 (2011) pp.1-1
ISSN: 1081-4159 eISSN: 1465-7325Published here -
Harris M, 'Longitudinal patterns of emerging literacy in beginning deaf and hearing readers'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16 (3) (2011) pp.289-304
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractThe emerging reading and spelling abilities of 24 deaf and 23 hearing beginning readers were followed over 2 years. The deaf children varied in their language backgrounds and preferred mode of communication. All children were given a range of literacy, cognitive and language-based tasks every 12 months. Deaf and hearing children made similar progress in literacy in the beginning stages of reading development and then their trajectories began to diverge. The longitudinal correlates of beginning reading in the deaf children were earlier vocabulary, letter-sound knowledge, and speechreading. Earlier phonological awareness was not a longitudinal correlate of reading ability once earlier reading levels were controlled. Only letter name knowledge was longitudinally related to spelling ability. Speechreading was also a strong longitudinal correlate of reading and spelling in the hearing children. The findings suggested that deaf and hearing children utilize slightly different reading strategies over the first 2 years of schooling.Published here -
Harris M, Terlektsi E, 'Reading and spelling abilities of deaf adolescents with cochlear implants and hearing aids'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16 (1) (2011) pp.24-34
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractPublished hereA total of 86 deaf children aged between 12 and 16 years were recruited from schools for the deaf, specialist units attached to a school, and mainstream schools. Approximately one-third used hearing aids, one-third had received a cochlear implant before 42 months, and one-third had been implanted later. The 3 subgroups were matched for age and nonverbal IQ, and all had an unaided hearing loss of at least 85 dB. Assessments revealed mean reading ages that were several years below chronological age for all 3 groups. However, participants in the hearing aid group performed best. Reading levels were not predicted by age of diagnosis or degree of hearing loss, but there was a relationship between reading level and presence of phonetic errors in spelling. There were also differences in educational setting, with the great majority of children in the hearing aid group in a school for the deaf and relatively more of the children with cochlear implants being educated in a unit or mainstream setting
-
Kyle FE, Harris M, 'Predictors of reading development in deaf children: A 3-year longitudinal study'
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 107 (3) (2010) pp.229-243
ISSN: 0022-0965AbstractPublished hereThe development of reading ability in a group of deaf children was followed over a 3-year period. A total of 29 deaf children (7-8 years of age at the first assessment) participated in the study, and every 12 months they were given a battery of literacy, cognitive, and language tasks. Earlier vocabulary and speechreading skills predicted longitudinal growth in reading achievement. The relations between reading and the predictor variables showed developmental change. Earlier reading ability was related to later phonological awareness skills, suggesting that deaf children might develop their phonological awareness through reading. Deaf children who had the most age-appropriate reading skills tended to have less severe hearing losses and earlier diagnoses and also preferred to communicate through speech. The theoretical implications of the role for speechreading, vocabulary and phonological awareness in deaf children" s literacy are discussed.
-
Harris M, ''I saw the real Father Christmas'! Children's everyday uses of the words real, really, and pretend'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 26 (3) (2008) pp.445-455
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835XAbstractThe words real, really and pretend are used in developmental research paradigms to reflect both the notions of 'authenticity' (in pretense-reality and appearance-reality research) and 'existence' (in fantasy-reality research). The current study explored whether children also expressed these notions in their everyday uses of real, really, and pretend. Mothers were interviewed and kept diary records, resulting in data for a younger group of children (2- to 3-year-olds, N = 80) and an older group (4- to 7-year-olds, N = 101). Utterances were analysed according to different uses of the target words and the topic of conversation in which they occurred. The words real, really, and pretend were used by both the younger and older age groups predominantly to consider the authenticity of things around them. Expression of the notion of existence was less common, particularly among the younger children. The implications of these findings are discussed in relation to children's performance in experimental studies, particularly those that intend to question children's understanding of the fantasy-reality distinction.Published here -
Archbold S, Harris M, O-™Donoghue G, Nikolopoulos T, White A, Lloyd Richmond H, 'Reading abilities after cochlear implantation: The effect of age at implantation on outcomes at 5 and 7 years after implantation '
International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 72 (10) (2008) pp.1471-1478
ISSN: 0165-5876AbstractObjectives The reading skills of deaf children have typically been delayed and this delay has been found to increase with age. This study explored the reading ability of a large group of children who had received cochlear implants 7 years earlier and investigated the relationship between reading ability and age at implantation. Methods The reading ages of 105 children, with age at implantation less than 7 years and onset of deafness below the age of three, were assessed 5 and 7 years after implantation using the Edinburgh reading test. Net reading age was calculated by using the difference between chronological age and reading age. Non-verbal intelligence was measured for a subset of 71 children, using Raven's coloured progressive matrices. Further investigation of this subset looked at the association of nonverbal intelligence, age at implantation and reading ability. Results There was a strong negative correlation at both 5 and 7 years after implant between net reading score and age at implantation. In the subset of 71 children who had an IQ score within normal range, those implanted at or before 42 months had age-appropriate reading both 5 and 7 years post-implant. This was not the case for children implanted after 42 months. Reading progress at the two post-implant assessment intervals were found to be highly related. Conclusions Age at implantation was a significant factor in the development of reading skills in this group. In children implanted below the age of 42 months, reading progress was in line with chronological age, which has not been the case previously with profoundly deaf children. With earlier implantation more common in present groups, and improved technology, there is every reason to be optimistic about the influence of cochlear implantation on the development of reading skills in deaf childrenPublished here -
Oates J M, Bard K, Harris M, 'Social and communicative functioning'
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 12 (3) (2008) pp.46-48
ISSN: 0968-7912AbstractIt is widely acknowledged that the establishment of positive attachment relationships and communication with primary caregivers is an important outcome of social-emotional development in early childhood. Attachment security and communication abilities are also associated with key developmental achievements later in childhood, and indeed across the life-span. Research conducted with children with Down syndrome suggests that although differences in attention regulation and emotional responsivity may modify the developmental processes in some respects, the general patterns are similar to those in typically-developing children. It is known from research in this latter population that sensitivity and"mind-mindedness" in caregiving are of key importance, as is the development of shared attentional focus. We argue that targeted research to identify the ways in which parents can most effectively support these core functions in the early development of children with Down syndrome should be a priority, as should involving parents as research partners in this endeavour.Published here -
Chasin J, Harris M, 'The development of visual attention in deaf children in relation to mother's hearing status'
Polish Psychological Bulletin 39 (1) (2008) pp.1-8
ISSN: 0079-2993AbstractPatterns of visual attention during free-play in deaf children with deaf (Dd) and hearing mothers (Dh) were compared at 9, 12 and 18 months. Dd children were more likely to look at their mother's face spontaneously than Dh children at all ages although spontaneous looking increased significantly at 18 months for both groups. The proportion of responsive looks declined at 12 months for the Dd group but not until 18 months for the Dh group. Elicited looking was more common in the Dd group and, at 12 months, a greater proportion of these looks were to the mother's face. Overall the results suggest that Dd children show greater sensitivity to the communicative significance of their mother's face in the second year of life.Published here -
Kyle FE, Harris M, 'Concurrent correlates and predictors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing school children.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11 (3) (2006) pp.273-88
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractSeven- and eight-year-old deaf children and hearing children of equivalent reading age were presented with a number of tasks designed to assess reading, spelling, productive vocabulary, speechreading, phonological awareness, short-term memory, and nonverbal intelligence. The two groups were compared for similarities and differences in the levels of performance and in the predictors of literacy. Multiple regressions showed that both productive vocabulary and speechreading were significant predictors of reading for the deaf children after hearing loss and nonverbal intelligence had been accounted for. However, spelling ability was not associated with any of the other measures apart from reading. For hearing children, age was the main determinant of reading and spelling ability (due to selection criterion). Possible explanations for the role of speechreading and productive vocabulary in deaf children's reading and the differences between the correlates of literacy for deaf and hearing children are discussed.Published here -
Harris M, Moreno C, 'Speech reading and learning to read: a comparison of 8-year-old profoundly deaf children with good and poor reading ability.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11 (2) (2006) pp.189-201
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractNine children with severe-profound prelingual hearing loss and single-word reading scores not more than 10 months behind chronological age (Good Readers) were matched with 9 children whose reading lag was at least 15 months (Poor Readers). Good Readers had significantly higher spelling and reading comprehension scores. They produced significantly more phonetic errors (indicating the use of phonological coding) and more often correctly represented the number of syllables in spelling than Poor Readers. They also scored more highly on orthographic awareness and were better at speech reading. Speech intelligibility was the same in the two groups. Cluster analysis revealed that only three Good Readers showed strong evidence of phonetic coding in spelling although seven had good representation of syllables; only four had high orthographic awareness scores. However, all 9 children were good speech readers, suggesting that a phonological code derived through speech reading may underpin reading success for deaf children.Published here -
Harris M, Chasin J, 'Visual attention in deaf and hearing infants: the role of auditory cues.'
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 46 (10) (2005) pp.1116-23
ISSN: 0021-9630AbstractBACKGROUND\nSuccessful communication with profoundly deaf children is heavily dependent on visual attention. Previous research has shown that mothers of deaf children--notably those who are deaf themselves--use a variety of strategies to gain their children's attention. This study compares patterns of visual attention in deaf and hearing children to determine how they are affected by the absence of auditory cues, especially when looking to the mother's face.\nMETHOD\nThe visual attention of 18-month-old infants to their mothers was examined in two groups of deaf children (6 with deaf mothers and 6 with hearing mothers) and two of hearing children (6 with deaf mothers and 8 with hearing mothers). Dyads were observed in free play and 10 minutes of videorecorded interaction was analysed. All looks to the mother were classified as Spontaneous, Responsive (Child turns in response to something done by mother) or Elicited (Mother actively seeks to gain child's attention by, e.g., tapping or waving). The kind of event that attracted the child's attention in Responsive and Elicited episodes was also determined (e.g., object movement, speech, physical contact), as was the focus of the child's attention (e.g., mother's face, mother's body).\nRESULTS\nResponsive looks to the mother were the most frequent for all groups but on only about 25% of occasions were they directed to her face. Elicited and spontaneous looks occurred less often but were frequently directed to the mother's face. Spontaneous looking occurred in all groups but elicited looking very seldom occurred in Hearing-Hearing dyads. Overall, there were fewer looks to the mother in the two groups where mother and child had congruent hearing status, although the proportion of the looks to the mother's face was similar.\nCONCLUSIONS\nBoth spontaneous and elicited looks are likely to involve attention to the mother's face. However, while active elicitation of attention is an important part of successful communication with young deaf children, this does not appear necessary for typically developing hearing children who turn to look at their mother's face on hearing her voice. The implications of these findings for differences in the dynamics of communication with young deaf and hearing children are discussed.Published here -
Harris M, Moreno C, 'Deaf children's use of phonological coding: evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 9 (3) (2004) pp.253-68
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractTwo groups of deaf children, aged 8 and 14 years, were presented with a number of tasks designed to assess their reliance on phonological coding. Their performance was compared with that of hearing children of the same chronological age (CA) and reading age (RA). Performance on the first task, short-term recall of pictures, showed that the deaf children's spans were comparable to those of RA controls but lower than CA controls. For the older deaf children, short-term memory span predicted reading ability. There was no clear evidence that the deaf children were using phonological coding in short-term memory when recall of dissimilar items was compared with recall of items with similarly sounding names. In the second task, which assessed orthographic awareness, performance of the deaf children was similar to that of RA controls although scores predicted reading level for the deaf children but not the hearing. The final task was a picture spelling test in which there were marked differences between the deaf and hearing children, most notably in the number of spelling refusals (which was higher for the deaf children in the older group than their RA controls) and the percentage of phonetic errors (which was considerably lower for both groups of deaf children than for any of the hearing controls). Overall these results provide support for the view that deaf children place little reliance on phonological coding.Published here -
Harris M, 'It's All a Matter of Timing: Sign Visibility and Sign Reference in Deaf and Hearing Mothers of 18-Month-Old Children.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 6 (3) (2001) pp.177-85
ISSN: 1465-7325AbstractThe signed and spoken language produced by 14 mothers to their 18-month-old children during free play was analyzed. All the children had profound prelingual deafness. Seven of the mothers were profoundly deaf and fluent users of British Sign Language (BSL) or Auslan. The other seven were hearing and had enrolled in a signing program. Maternal signed utterances were classified according to whether they were made in the child's line of sight and whether they had a salient context; that is, they referred to an object or event at the child's current focus of attention. Spoken utterances were coded by word length. Comparisons between the two groups showed that both deaf and hearing mothers produced a majority of single-sign utterances (rather than utterances containing two or more signs). Deaf mothers also produced a majority of single-word spoken utterances, whereas the hearing mothers produced a significantly greater proportion of multiword utterances. As predicted, deaf mothers were more successful than hearing mothers in presenting signed utterances with a salient context that were visible to their children. Across the group as a whole, the total number of visible and salient signed utterances produced in 10 minutes was positively correlated with the total number of occasions on which mothers successfully redirected their child's attention or the child spontaneously turned to look at the mother. This suggests that deaf children who are visually attentive to their mothers receive a greater number of visible signed utterances with a salient context. I argue that this provides a more secure context for early language development.Published here -
Harris M, Beech J, 'Implicit phonological awareness and early reading development in prelingually deaf children.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 3 (3) (1998) pp.205-16
ISSN: 1465-7325AbstractThis article reports on a longitudinal study of reading progress in a group of five-year-old deaf children and a group of hearing controls. All children were prereaders at the beginning of the study and the IQ of the two groups were matched. The deaf children varied considerably on a number of measures, including implicit phonological awareness, oral ability, and familiarity with British Sign Language and fingerspelling. Overall, the deaf children made significantly less reading progress than their hearing peers over the first year of schooling, and they also scored significantly lower on the test of rime and onset awareness. However, considerable variation in the reading progress of the deaf children was positively correlated with oral skills, rime/onset awareness, and language comprehension. Language comprehension, itself, was positively correlated with signing and fingerspelling. The deaf children were assessed again one year later, when learning to read continued to be very delayed, and the pattern of correlation was essentially the same. The implications of these findings for the education of deaf children are discussed. -
Harris M, Mohay H, 'Learning to look in the right place: a comparison of attentional behavior in deaf children with deaf and hearing mothers.'
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2 (2) (1997) pp.95-103
ISSN: 1081-4159AbstractEleven 18-month-old children with profound prelingual hearing loss were video-recorded in a free-play session with their mothers. Five of the mothers were profoundly deaf and fluent users of British Sign Language (BSL) or Auslan. The other six were hearing and had enrolled in a signing program. Ten-minute segments from each session were analyzed to determine the number of switches in attention shown by each child. Switches in attention were subdivided into three categories: spontaneous (where the child spontaneously looked to the mother); responsive (where the child responded to some maternal action such as moving an object); and elicited (where the mother made a direct attempt to gain the child's attention. Failed attempts to gain attention were also noted. A comparison of deaf and hearing mothers revealed no difference in the proportion of spontaneous or responsive switches in attention shown by their children. Responsive switches were by far the most frequent category for both groups, but these most commonly focused on objects and did not provide an opportunity for maternal signing. Successful perception of signing typically followed from spontaneous or elicited attentional switches. Deaf mothers were generally more insistent on their children turning to look at them, and they were more successful in eliciting attentional switches although they also had more failed attempts. These overall differences between the two groups were overshadowed by large individual differences within the groups. Within the sample there were both deaf and hearing mothers who achieved successful signed communication with their children. -
Harris M, Yeeles C, Chasin J, Oakley Y, 'Symmetries and asymmetries in early lexical comprehension and production.'
Journal of Child Language 22 (1) (1995) pp.1-18
ISSN: 0305-0009AbstractSix children were visited in their homes every two weeks for 18 months from the age of six months in order to observe their developing comprehension and production of words. Controlled testing of the children's developing vocabulary was also carried out to provide a more detailed picture of the precise context in which a word was understood or produced. The results showed both similarities and individual differences among the children in patterns of early comprehension. For all six children there was a very close relationship between early production and comprehension of words: words that were contextually flexible in production tended also to be so in comprehension and words that were context-bound also tended to be so in both modalities although there was some evidence that the two types of word differed in the extent to which comprehension preceded production. All children were also similar in showing evidence of contextually flexible comprehension very early on. However, there were individual differences both in the rate at which comprehension developed and in the lag between comprehension and production. The significance of this pattern of development for accounts of lexical development is explored. -
Barrett M, Harris M, Chasin J, 'Early lexical development and maternal speech: a comparison of children's initial and subsequent uses of words.'
Journal of Child Language 18 (1) (1991) pp.21-40
ISSN: 0305-0009AbstractIn Harris, Barrett, Jones & Brookes (1988), we reported the results of a detailed analysis of the initial uses of the first 10 words which were produced by four children. The present paper reports the results of an analysis of the subsequent uses of these 40 words. This analysis reveals that seven qualitatively different patterns of change occurred between the children's initial and subsequent uses of these words; the particular patterns of change which occurred support Barrett's (1986) model of early lexical development. In addition, it was found that, although there was a strong relationship between maternal speech and the children's initial word uses, the relationship between maternal speech and the children's subsequent word uses was very much weaker. These findings indicate that the role of linguistic input in early lexical development may decline quite sharply once the child has established initial uses for words. -
van der Lely HK, Harris M, 'Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children.'
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 55 (1) (1990) pp.101-17
ISSN: 0022-4677AbstractThis study investigated comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired (SLI) children. Two experiments, using different paradigms, were undertaken. In Experiment 1, 14 SLI children (aged 4:10-7:10) were compared with children matched on chronological age and language age (LA). Subjects acted out 36 semantically reversible sentences that varied in thematic content (transitives, locatives, and datives) and in the order of thematic roles (canonical and noncanonical). The SLI children performed at a significantly lower level than both control groups. In Experiment 2, the same sentences were presented using a picture-pointing task. A single word vocabulary test preceded the test sentences to assess semantic knowledge of the predicates. Sixteen SLI children were compared with language age controls. No significant differences were found between the performance of the two groups on the vocabulary test, and in general, the results of Experiment 2 supported those of Experiment 1. Analysis of individual children's error patterns identified qualitative differences between the SLI children and the LA controls. The majority of SLI children had a very high proportion of word order errors. The proportion of word order errors of the SLI children, unlike those of the LA controls, was unrelated to language age. These findings are considered in relation to the processes involved in sentence comprehension. -
Harris M, Barrett M, Jones D, Brookes S, 'Linguistic input and early word meaning.'
Journal of Child Language 15 (1) (1988) pp.77-94
ISSN: 0305-0009 -
HARRIS M, JONES D, BROOKES S, GRANT J, 'RELATIONS BETWEEN THE NONVERBAL CONTEXT OF MATERNAL SPEECH AND RATE OF LANGUAGE-DEVELOPMENT'
British Journal of Developmental Psychology 4 (1986) pp.261-268
ISSN: 0261-510X eISSN: 2044-835X -
Nicolaci-da-Costa A, Harris M, 'Redundancy of syntactic information: an aid to young children's comprehension of sentential number.'
British Journal of Psychology 74 (Pt 3) (1983) pp.343-52
ISSN: 0007-1269AbstractSentences conveying either non-redundant or increasingly redundant information about number, by means of different syntactic markers, were presented to 20 nursery school children. Comprehension of each sentence was tested in two tasks: pointing to one of a pair of line drawings and acting out with toys the action described by a sentence. The results strongly supported the view that redundancy of syntactic information is a powerful aid to children's comprehension of sentential number. In a second experiment, comprehension of sentences conveying redundant information by repetition of the same number marker was compared with that of non-redundant sentences. In contrast to the first experiment, redundancy had no significant effect on comprehension. Together, these results suggest that children's comprehension of sentential number if aided by redundancy of information involving different syntactic markers, but not by mere repetition of the same marker.
Books
-
Harris M, Westermann G, Kim S, Bazhydai M, A Student's Guide to Developmental Psychology, Routledge Taylor and Francis group (2024)
ISBN: 9780367471040AbstractPublished hereNow in its second edition, this fundamental undergraduate textbook provides students with everything they need when studying developmental psychology.
Thoroughly revised, this book breaks down key topics into easily accessible concepts and provides students with both an overview of traditional research and theory as well as an insight into the latest research findings and techniques. Taking a chronological approach, the key milestones from birth to adolescence are highlighted and clear links between changes in behaviour and developments in brain activity are made. A new chapter provides a global perspective on development, including findings regarding children’s motor, cognitive, literacy, social and emotional development, as well as the importance of cross-cultural studies and their challenges. Each chapter also highlights both typical and atypical developments, as well as discussing and contrasting the effects of genetic and environmental factors.
This textbook comes with a wealth of carefully updated pedagogical features, designed to help students engage with the material, including:
• Learning objectives for every chapter
• Key term definitions
• Over 100 colour illustrations
• Chapter summaries
• Further reading
• Suggested essay questions.A Student’s Guide to Developmental Psychology is accompanied by a support material package, featuring a range of helpful supplementary resources including exclusive video clips to illustrate key developmental concepts, multiple-choice questions, flashcards and more.
This book is essential reading for all undergraduate students of developmental psychology. It will also be of interest to those in education, healthcare and other subjects requiring an up-to-date and accessible overview of child development. -- Provided by publisher.
-
Harris M, Exploring developmental psychology: Understanding theory and methods , Sage (2008)
ISBN: 9781412903356AbstractA wide range of approaches are used to investigate children's development. This book, aimed at advanced undergraduates and postgraduate students in psychology and related areas, provides a guide to the key theories and methods used by researchers. Carefully chosen articles are accompanied by a commentary from the author that helps students to understand the rationale for a study, the choice of design and assessment measures, use of statistics, and the interpretation of results. A wide range of recent research papers is included to cover observational and experimental methods from infancy to adolescence. The research papers are introduced by two chapters that consider the relationship between theory and methods, explaining how models of development differ from one another, and how they can be tested through experimental studies. This innovative text is perfect for all advanced students taking courses in developmental psychology or child development.
Book chapters
-
Harris M, 'The impact of cochlear implants on deaf children's literacy' in Marschark M, Spencer PE (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies in Language, Oxford University Press (2016)
ISBN: 9780190241414 eISBN: 9780190297299AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThis chapter reviews recent studies looking at the impact of cochlear implants on the development of reading and writing of deaf children. Although there is good evidence that cochlear implants have significantly improved the spoken language of many children with severe-profound hearing loss, their impact on literacy – something that deaf children have traditionally found challenging – has proved to be considerably less consistent. In particular, benefits that are evident in the early years of education are often reduced as children progress through school. The aim of the chapter is to identify factors that affect the impact of cochlear implants at different stages of learning to read and write by considering both the skills that underpin early and later literacy for deaf children and factors affecting the efficacy of cochlear implants. The chapter concludes with a consideration of how the literacy development of deaf children with cochlear implants can best be supported through the use of interventions that enable the development of robust phonological coding. Part Six. Chapter 27.
-
Harris M, 'Acquisition of sign language' in The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the Language Sciences, Cambridge University Press (2011)
ISBN: 9780521866897 -
Harris M, 'Acquisition of writing and reading' in The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the Language Sciences, Cambridge University Press (2011)
ISBN: 9780521866897 -
Harris M, 'Early communication in sign and speech' in Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education, Oxford University Press (2010)
ISBN: 9780195390032
Other publications
-
Carrol S, Walsh C, Burgess M, Harris M, Mansbridge S, King N, Bunce L, 'Assessing the viability of EVs in daily life', (2014)
Published here -
Harris M, 'The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension.', (1999)
-
Harris M, 'Language learning: A special case for developmental psychology? - Howe,CJ', (1996)
-
Harris M, 'The child's path to spoken language - Locke,JR', (1996)
-
Marschark M, Harris M, 'Success and failure in learning to read: The special case (?) of deaf children', (1996)
-
BARRETT M, HARRIS M, 'SPECIAL ISSUE - LANGUAGE AND COGNITION IN EARLY SOCIAL-INTERACTION', (1986)
-
HARRIS M, 'FUNCTIONAL-APPROACH TO CHILD LANGUAGE - STUDY OF DETERMINERS AND REFERENCE - KARMILOFFSMITH,A', (1980)