Professor Kate Wilmut
BSc, PhD
Professor of Psychology
School of Psychology, Social Work and Public Health
Role
I am the co-director for the Centre for Psychological Research and the Postgraduate Tutor for Research Students. In terms of my role with the Centre for Psychological Research I help to steer the strategic direction of research within the centre and help to build research capacity which feeds into my role of Postgraduate Tutor for Research Students. I also have a number of teaching roles detailed below.
My research interests focus on all aspects of human movement and motor control and I am part of the Perception and Motion Analysis (PuMA) lab. My primary expertise is in individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) / Dyspraxia but I also work in other areas such as movement across the lifespan and the role of play and physical education in movement. You can find more information about these areas below.
To listen to a podcast where I am interviewed about my research please visit https://perceptionaction.com/199/
Teaching and supervision
Courses
Modules taught
UG teaching:
- Perception and Action
- The Psychology of Motor Skill and Play
- Understanding Developmental Disorders
- Project module leadership and supervision
PG teaching:
- Writing and Critical Analysis Skills
- Dissertation Project Supervision
Supervision
Previous supervision includes:
- Dr. Catherine Purcell (completed May 2012): aspects of road crossing in children with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder.
- Dr. Mellissa Prunty (completed May 2014): handwriting skills in children with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder.
- Dr. Judith Gentle (completed Feb 2015): aspects of gait in children and adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder.
Current supervision includes:
- Kim Griffin, Auditing the impact online sensory processing trainning has on school
Research Students
Name | Thesis title | Completed |
---|---|---|
Fiyory Ghezae | Nonlinear pedagogy to support children in play and physical activity | Active |
Sophie Harris | The influence of anxiety, self-efficacy and movement variability on the perceptual judgements and executed actions of adults with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) | Active |
Julie Bennett | Experiences of nostalgia in individuals both with and without Autism | 2024 |
Research
My research interests focus on all aspects of human movement and motor control and I am part of the Perception and Motion Analysis (PuMA) lab.
My primary expertise is in individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) / Dyspraxia and I have worked in this area since 2001. Initially my research focused on understanding and describing the motor difficulties of this population. This built up my expertise in using 3D motion analysis systems to measure and analyse movement. More recently I have undertaken work which focuses more on the lived experience of this individuals in terms of road-crossing, physical activity and wellbeing.
Other strands of my research include:
- Movement across the lifespan where I have considered the role of cognitive function in motor decline and road-crossing experiences.
- The role of play, physical education and physical activity in wellbeing
- Neurological underpinnings of both DCD and ADHD
More details about these projects can be found on the Perception and Motion Analysis webpages.
I am always interested in new collaborations and in supervising students with an interest in any of the areas detailed above. Please do get in touch or follow me on twitter.
Research grants and awards
External funding:
- Wilmut, K. & Purcell, C. The Bikeability Trust (£10,000) Evaluation of SEND cycle training. Awarded 2021
- Wilmut, K. The Leverhulme Trust (£52,582) Movement planning during reach-to-grasp tasks across the lifespan. Awarded 2016
- Wilmut, K. and Barnett, A. L. ESRC (£280,261) Navigational abilities of typically developing individuals and individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Awarded 2012
- Wilmut, K. ESRC (£166,515) Forward anticipation of movement in typically developing children and children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Awarded 2010
- Wann, J. P and Wilmut, K. ESRC (£386,348) Judgments of vehicle approach, in a road crossing situation, amongst primary school children. Awarded 2007
- Wann, J. P and Wilmut, K. ESRC (£43,452) The role of predictive information during goal-directed action in children. Awarded 2005.
Internal funding:
- Wilmut, K (£19,156.22) Cycle training for all: Evaluation of instructor training to ensure inclusivity
- Wilmut, K. (£20,000) Motor learning for coordinative tasks in Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Awarded 2017
- Barnett, A. & Wilmut, K. (£6,456) Assessment of motor ability in adulthood. Awarded 2011
- Wilmut,K. & Barnett, A. (£2,165) Navigational ability of children moving through apertures. Awarded 2009
- Wilmut, K. & Barnett, A. (£3,492) Construction and maintenance of a database of individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Awarded 2008
- Barnett, A. & Wilmut, K. (£4,986) Locomotor behaviour of adults while passing through apertures. Awarded 2007
Centres and institutes
Groups
Projects as Principal Investigator, or Lead Academic if project is led by another Institution
- Setting the Research Priorities for Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) in the UK (led by Cardiff University) (01/09/2024 - 31/08/2025), funded by: The Waterloo Foundation
Publications
Journal articles
-
Harris S, Rathbone C, Wilmut K, 'The role of anxiety and self-efficacy in movement'
Human Movement Science 98 (2024)
ISSN: 0167-9457 eISSN: 1872-7646AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrevious research suggests that affective factors may influence perception of potential movement differently compared to perception during movement itself. To build on this the current study investigated the roles of general and movement-specific anxiety, self-efficacy, general resilience and motor control in how 41 adults with typical motor skills thought they would behave (perceptual judgement) and how they actually behaved (executed action). Participants completed several standardised scales and two movement-specific scales, a perceptual judgement task and an executed action task. In the perceptual judgement task participants judged whether they would need to turn their shoulders to walk through different sized apertures between 0.9 and 1.9 their shoulder width-to-aperture ratio. This involved a static (standing still) and a dynamic (walking towards) condition. The executed action task involved actually walking through the different sized gaps between the doors. Findings were discussed within an ecological framework drawing strongly on Newell’s constraints-based approach (1986). Results indicated a relationship between higher movement-specific anxiety and bigger safety margins. This highlights the importance of measure specificity in being able to detect nuanced relationships between affective factors and the perception-action cycle. Notable differences were also shown in the point of behaviour change (critical ratio) between perceptual judgement and executed action, illustrating the importance of studying perception and action together since they can be subject to different constraints. The findings contribute novel insights into the roles of these factors in how adults with typical motor skills perceive and realise their intentions and abilities to act in the world.
-
McQueen M, Parker A, Pascoe M, Baldwin P, Mancini V, Cairney J, Wilmut K, Williams J, 'Investigating the Feasibility and Acceptability of a Facebook Delivered, Parent Mediated, Physical Activity Intervention for Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education [Online first] (2024)
ISSN: 1034-912X eISSN: 1465-346XAbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARChildren with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) experience difficulties performing fundamental movement skills, resulting in reduced physical activity (PA). Given low PA can impact mental and physical health, improving PA in DCD appears imperative. This study investigates the feasibility of a Facebook delivered, parent mediated, PA intervention for children with DCD. Parents of children with DCD (n=31; 5-11 years), were invited to private Facebook groups where physical activity resources and questionnaires were provided over 12 weeks. Parents and their children wore activity trackers during the intervention period. Feasibility and acceptability of the intervention was considered with reference to (1) Facebook engagement, (2) adherence intervention components, and (3) parent perspectives. Facebook engagement was high, with 78.03% (SD=23.06) of all intervention posts being viewed by parents on average. Activity trackers were worn correctly for 92.45% of intervention days and 95.61% of intervention weeks. Weekly questionnaires were completed, on average, 84.37% of the time. Parent perspectives were positive, with 80.77% somewhat or strongly agreeing that the intervention was useful for themselves and their child. Given the results described, Facebook appears to be a feasible platform through which parent mediated physical activity interventions can be provided to children with DCD.
-
Bennett J, Wilmut K, Rathbone CJ, 'Music evoked nostalgia and wellbeing in autistic and non-autistic young adults'
International Journal of Applied Positive Psychology [Online first] (2024)
ISSN: 2364-5040 eISSN: 2364-5059AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARNostalgia is known to boost state wellbeing and authenticity for people in the general population. However, little research has explored nostalgia’s benefits outside of the general population. This study had two aims: 1) to explore music evoked nostalgia and its effects on mood, state wellbeing and authenticity in autistic individuals, a group of people who tend to experience reduced wellbeing, compared to non-autistic individuals and 2) to examine the influence of dispositional rumination and reflection on any benefits gained from nostalgic recollection. 146 autistic (69 diagnosed and 77 who self-identified as autistic) and 174 non-autistic people aged 18 – 25 took part in an online survey. Participants completed scales measuring autistic traits, rumination and reflection. They also reported their mood before, and their mood, state wellbeing and authenticity after a nostalgia manipulation, in which they either listened to and reflected on a nostalgic or a non-nostalgic song. Music evoked nostalgia (versus control) boosted state wellbeing and authenticity in autistic as well as non-autistic individuals. Results also indicated that people who were prone to reflection versus rumination were more likely to reap wellbeing benefits of engaging in nostalgia. This study is the first to show that music evoked nostalgia boosts state wellbeing and authenticity in autistic as well as in non-autistic individuals. Findings suggest that, depending on individual tendencies towards rumination and/or reflection, nostalgia could be viewed as a positive activity that autistic people could use to support wellbeing, aiding their day to day living in the non-autistic social world.
-
Purcell C, Dahl A, Gentle J, Hill E, Kirby A, Mason A, McQuillan V, Meek A, Payne S, Scott-Roberts S, Shaw K, Wilmut K
, 'Harnessing real-life experiences: The development of guidelines to communicate research findings on Developmental Coordination Disorder / Dyspraxia'
Research Involvement and Engagement 10 (2024)
ISSN: 2056-7529 eISSN: 2056-7529AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDevelopmental Coordination Disorder (DCD), also known as dyspraxia, affects 5-15% of school-aged children (Hamilton & Sutton, 2002) and significantly impacts a child’s ability to learn motor skills and perform everyday activities efficiently and effectively (Zwicker et al., 2012). These motor deficits can have a negative impact on academic performance, vocational choices and leisure pursuits (Zwicker et al., 2012) and profoundly impact quality of life (Izadi-Najafabadi et al., 2019). DCD persists into adulthood (Kirby et al., 2011), impacting motor as well as emotional and behavioural status (Tal Saban & Kirby, 2018). Despite the continued increase in research in the field of DCD, awareness of DCD remains poor (O’Kelly, 2022) even though it has higher prevalence rates when compared to, for example, autism spectrum disorder (Yan et al., 2024), which in part may be due to a lack of accessible research findings. A fundamental feature of the research process is disseminating research findings. This should involve community members in design and delivery to ensure the accessibility of research findings.
In 2022 the DCD-UK committee established a DCD Research Advisory Group (DCD-RAG) which met over the course of 12 months to: (1) identify issues of inaccessible research findings; (2) determine the need for a repository for research summaries; (3) co-create guidelines for authors and (4) agree a process for reviewing research summaries to be housed on the Movement Matters website. The new co-produced research repository, author guidelines and process were launched at the DCD-UK conference in Manchester 2023 and subsequently shared on social media and through the DCD research email list. The creation of the DCD-RAG and the process that we undertook together to create a non-academic repository for DCD research summaries are described. It is hoped that this repository will enable the wider public, community members and professionals to be able to readily benefit from accessible research, increasing a deeper and broader understanding of the evidence in the field.
-
Bennett J, Rathbone C, Wilmut K, 'Nostalgia, authenticity and wellbeing in autistic and non-autistic young adults'
Current Psychology [online first] (2024)
ISSN: 1046-1310 eISSN: 1936-4733AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARResearch in the general population has shown that nostalgia is associated with greater wellbeing through increased authenticity. It is well documented that autistic versus non-autistic individuals experience reduced wellbeing. This is the first study to explore nostalgia in autistic people and whether nostalgia might support the wellbeing of autistic people in the same way as non-autistic. This study had two aims: (i) to explore the autistic experience of nostalgia (proneness, triggers of nostalgia, related affect) and (ii) to examine the interrelationships at trait level between nostalgia, authenticity and wellbeing in autistic and non-autistic young adults. Participants (n = 230) completed an online questionnaire and reported about their nostalgic experiences, wellbeing, authenticity and autistic traits. Participants also wrote about either a nostalgic or an ordinary memory and rated how happy they felt after reflecting on the memory. With regards to the interrelationships at trait level within the whole cohort, nostalgia was not associated with increased authenticity or wellbeing. In contrast, authenticity was associated with increased wellbeing. However, nostalgia was not associated with greater wellbeing through increased authenticity. Differences in these interrelationships between autistic and non-autistic individuals are also discussed. The autistic experience of nostalgia was similar to the non-autistic, with the exception that autistic participants were less prone to nostalgia. At state level, findings also suggested that reflecting on nostalgic memories may boost positive affect in autistic as well as non-autistic individuals. This study may have implications for boosting wellbeing in autistic people.
-
Meachon EJ, Kundlacz M, Wilmut K, Alpers GW, 'EEG spectral power in developmental coordination disorder and attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder: A pilot study'
Frontiers in Psychology 15 (2024)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDevelopmental coordination disorder (DCD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) overlap in symptoms and often co-occur. Differentiation of DCD and ADHD is crucial for a better understanding of the conditions and targeted support. Measuring electrical brain activity with EEG may help to discern and better understand the conditions given that it can objectively capture changes and potential differences in brain activity related to externally measurable symptoms beneficial for targeted interventions. Therefore, a pilot study was conducted to exploratorily examine neurophysiological differences between adults with DCD and/or ADHD at rest. A total of N = 46 adults with DCD (n = 12), ADHD (n = 9), both DCD+ADHD (n = 8), or typical development (n = 17) completed two minutes of rest with eyes-closed and eyes-open while their EEG was recorded. Spectral power was calculated for frequency bands: delta (0.5-3 Hz), theta (3.5-7 Hz), alpha (7.5-12.5 Hz), beta (13-25 Hz), mu (8-13 Hz), gamma (low: 30-40 Hz; high: 40-50 Hz). Within-participants, spectral power in a majority of waveforms significantly increased from eyes-open to eyes-closed conditions. Groups differed significantly in occipital beta power during the eyes-open condition, driven by the DCD versus typically developing group comparison. However, other group comparisons reached only marginal significance, including whole brain alpha and mu power with eyes-open, and frontal beta and occipital high gamma power during eyes-closed. While no strong markers could be determined to differentiate DCD versus ADHD, we theorize that several patterns in beta activity were indicative of potential motor maintenance differences in DCD at rest. Therefore, larger studies comparing EEG spectral power may be useful to identify neurological mechanisms of DCD and continued differentiation of DCD and ADHD.
-
Falemban R, Wilmut K, Hurst H, Purcell C, 'Roadside Experiences of Parents of Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder and/or Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder'
Frontiers in Psychology 18 (2024)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPedestrians are a vulnerable group at the roadside and previous research has identified that children with DCD and ADHD are at a heightened risk of pedestrian injuries. Despite this, limited research has explored parental perspectives of the pedestrian risks faced by children with DCD and/or ADHD. Understanding parents’ perspectives provides a unique insight into the challenges children face every day and the concerns that parents perceive regarding their children’s safety as pedestrians. Therefore, the aim of this study was to explore parents’ perspectives of the pedestrian risks faced by their children with DCD and/or ADHD.
Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 14 parents of primary school and early secondary school aged children with age range 7- 17 (3 DCD, 5 ADHD and 6 both). All parents confirmed an existing diagnosis and completed the SNAP-IV and DCDQ as screening tools. The interviews explored parents’ perspectives regarding their children’s pedestrian behaviours, parents’ concerns and preventative measures taken to improve the pedestrian safety of their children with DCD and/or ADHD. Reflexive thematic analysis was undertaken to analyse the interviews, from which three themes were developed.
The first theme related to the challenges experienced by children at the roadside; parents emphasised the significance of structured and controlled pedestrian crossing sites, underlining their preference for designated crossings as safer options due to their heightened perceptions of risk associated with other road-crossing locations. The second theme: parental concerns and influences on children’s road safety referred to their children’s performance and safety at the roadside, leading to increased monitoring and a more protective approach to road crossing. The third theme: road safety education related to various strategies parents implemented to mitigate risks, whilst balancing independence and prioritising their safety. While there were commonalities in the challenges faced by children with DCD and/or ADHD at the roadside, there were also notable differences. Parents of children with DCD discussed challenges with spatial awareness and motor skills, whereas parents of children with ADHD discussed challenges with impulsivity and inattention. Parents of children with co-occurring DCD and ADHD described a complex interplay of these challenges.
It is evident from the interviews that children with DCD and/or ADHD require a distinct approach to develop their pedestrian skills effectively and parents reported specific strategies they used to address the risks associated with their children’s roadside behaviour. Promoting pedestrian safety for children with DCD and/or ADHD necessitates collaboration among parents, schools and local authorities to implement comprehensive measures ensuring their safety. These findings contribute to understanding parental experiences and needs, providing valuable guidance for targeted interventions and policies to enhance the road safety of children with DCD and/or ADHD.
-
Zhang S, Wilmut K, Zhang K, Wang S, 'Age-related changes in motor planning for prior intentions: A mouse tracking reach-to-click task'
Frontiers in Psychology [online first] (2024)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished hereWhen we complete sequential movements with different intentions, we plan our movements and
adjust ahead. Such a phenomenon is called anticipatory planning for prior intentions and is known to
decline with age. In daily life activities, we often need to consider and plan for multiple demands in
one movement sequence. However, previous studies only considered one dimension of prior
intentions, either different types of onward actions or different precisions of fit or placement.
Therefore, in this study, we investigated anticipatory planning for both extrinsic (movement
direction) and intrinsic (fit precision) target-related properties in a computer-based movement task
and analysed the computer cursor movement kinematics of both young and older adults. We found
that older people consider and adjust for different properties step-by-step, with movement direction
being considered as a prior intention during reach movement and fit precision as a motor constraint
during drop movement. The age-related changes in the completion of onward actions are constrained
by one’s general cognitive ability, sensorimotor performance and effective motor planning for prior
intentions. -
Wilmut K, Purcell C, Meachon E, 'Current perspectives on Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD): volume II'
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience (2024)
ISSN: 1662-5161 eISSN: 1662-5161AbstractThis is an editorial and so does not have an abstract
-
Purcell C, Schott N, Rapos V, Zwicker JG, Wilmut K, 'Understanding factors that influence physical activity behavior in people with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD): a mixed methods convergent integrated systematic review'
Frontiers in Psychology 17 (2023)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThis systematic review synthesizes the literature on physical activity amongst people with DCD using the COM-B framework. The review questions were: (1) what is the Capability (C), Opportunity (O) and Motivation (M) for physical activity and (2) what does physical activity behavior (B) look like? A mixed-methods systematic review was conducted by searching eight databases (PubMed, APA PsycINFO, EMBASE, Scopus, Child Development and Adolescent Studies, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, CINAHL) up to July 2023. Data were extracted, thematically analyzed, and mapped to the COM-B model. The quality of studies was assessed with the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) critical appraisal tool. The protocol was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42022319127). Forty-three papers, 42 of which related to children, were included. Fifteen aligned with physical activity behavior, nine with physical capability, thirteen with psychological capability, one with social opportunity, one with physical opportunity, one with reflective motivation and three with automatic motivation. Pre-school-aged children with DCD engage in comparable levels of physical activity behavior, but differences emerge from 6 years of age. Characteristics of DCD result in reduced physical capability and less varied participation in physical activity. This impacts psychological capability, whereby lower self-perceptions result in a negative feedback loop and reduce the
motivation to participate. Barriers relating to social opportunities may result in poor reflective and
automatic motivation, although there is evidence that interventions can enhance enjoyment in the
short term. -
Wilmut K, Purcell C, 'Parent perceptions of cycle training for children with Special Education Needs and Disabilities: what drives intention to cycle?'
Heliyon 9 (5) (2023)
ISSN: 2405-8440 eISSN: 2405-8440AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBackground: Young people with Special Educational Needs and Disabilities (SEND) are especially vulnerable to physical inactivity. Research has demonstrated the effectiveness of tailored cycle training for children with SEND, however, it is unclear whether this then results in a greater uptake of cycling.
Aims: To determine parent based perceptions of a SEND cycle training programme, factors which predict intention to cycle more and ongoing barriers to cycling.
Methods: A tailored questionnaire was distributed to parents of children who took part in the cycle training.
Results: Parents reported increased confidence in their child's ability to cycle independently with many also highlighting improvements in confidence and resilience. The outcome of the cycle training (level of enjoyment and change in cycling ability) had a positive influence on intention to cycle more while cycling frequency prior to training had a negative influence. Continuing barriers to cycling were identified which included difficulties accessing specialised equipment and a need for additional on-road cycle training.
Conclusions and implications: This study demonstrates the success of a specialised cycle training programme for children with SEND in terms of improvement to cycling and influences on the intention to cycle more.
-
Krajenbrink H, Lust J, Wilmut K, Steenbergen B, 'Motor and cognitive dual-task performance under low and high task complexity in children with and without developmental coordination disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 135 (2023)
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBackground.
In everyday life, tasks are often performed simultaneously, which may be more difficult for children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) than their peers.
Aims.
To examine (1) the effects of task complexity and type of concurrent task on dual-task performance in children with and without DCD; and (2) if the amount of effort that children put into the task performance differs between the groups.
Methods.
Participants were 64 children with and without DCD (aged 7–14 years). The dual-task paradigm consisted of a manual dexterity task of relatively low complexity (box and block test) or relatively high complexity (pegboard task), and a concurrent motor task (cycling task) or a concurrent cognitive task (word-listening task). To assess mental effort, children were asked how tired they felt before and after the experiment.
Results.
Dual-task interference was highest when the manual dexterity task of relatively high complexity was combined with the concurrent motor task. There were no group differences in dual-task interference, but children with DCD reported a larger increase in the level of tiredness after the experiment indicative of greater mental effort.
Conclusions.
Depending on task demands, children with DCD are able to perform dual-tasks at the same level as their peers, but performance may take children with DCD more mental effort.
-
Harris S, Purcell C, Wilmut K, 'Moving with confidence: How does anxiety impede performance in individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD)?'
Current Developmental Disorders Reports 9 (2022) pp.98-104
ISSN: 2196-2987 eISSN: 2196-2987AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPurpose of review: It is well-established that anxiety levels are higher among people with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) compared to their peers. However, it is unclear whether this anxiety influences movement and behaviour. The purpose of this paper is to review studies demonstrating the influence of anxiety on movement and/or behaviour in non-DCD and DCD populations.
Recent findings: When considering non-DCD literature, many studies have illustrated the influence of anxiety on movement. Only two papers were found which explicitly aimed to consider the influence of anxiety on movement in a DCD population. These findings are described and explored against a backdrop of the wider research field.
Summary: This paper has highlighted the potential role of anxiety in constraining movement patterns in DCD. While a great deal more evidence is needed before definitive conclusions there is emerging evidence that motor behaviour may be related to task-specific anxiety in children with DCD.
-
Meachon EJ, Beitz C, Zemp M, Wilmut K, Alpers GW, 'The Adult Developmental Coordination Disorders/Dyspraxia Checklist - German: Adapted factor structure for the differentiation of DCD and ADHD'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 126 (2022)
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBackground: The Adult Dyspraxia/DCD Checklist (ADC) is the only existing self-report questionnaire to screen adults for potential Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). It was developed in English and Hebrew, however, its factor structure has not yet been assessed.
Aims: The goals of the current study were to (1) develop and refine a German translation, (2) explore the emerging factors in a new and restructured ADC in German and apply this structure to an English ADC, and (3) explore its potential for distinguishing DCD versus ADHD.
Method: In a series of three studies, we assessed comprehensibility of the translation and revisions to the questionnaire. We further examined subscale structure in a sample of N=148 individuals with DCD or ADHD and retested it in an English-speaking sample (N=134).
Results: Overall, we found decent reliability and construct validity for the German ADC. Three components emerged with themes of fine motor coordination, gross motor coordination, and executive functions which had strong psychometric properties in German and English.
Conclusions: The studies collectively highlight the German translation is effective and has strong potential to differentiate DCD and ADHD. Most notably, there are unique symptom profiles in motor and executive functioning difficulties in adults with DCD or ADHD.
-
Wilmut K, Williams J, Purcell C, 'Editorial: Current Perspectives in Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD)'
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16 (2022)
ISSN: 1662-5161 eISSN: 1662-5161Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, Wang S, Barnett A, 'Inter-limb coordination in a novel pedalo task: a comparison of children with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder '
Human Movement Science 82 (2022)
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARChildren with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) have been shown to have different coordination patterns on some tasks compared to their typically developing peers. However, it is unclear whether these differences are driven by the fact that typically developing children tend to be more practiced at the task on which coordination is being measured. The current study used a novel pedalo task to measure coordination in order to eliminate any practice differences. Thirty children (8 years -16 years), 15 with DCD and 15 without were recruited for this study. Children pedalled along an 8m line 20 times. Movement of the 7th Cervical Vertebra, shoulders, elbows, wrists, hips, knees, ankles and toes was recorded. In terms of outcome measures, pedalling speed was not different between the groups but the coefficient of variation of speed was higher in the children with DCD indicating a less smooth movement. Coordination was measured by calculating angles at the shoulder, elbow, hip, knee and ankle. A higher correlation coefficient (more tightly coupled movement) and a greater variation in joint angle was seen in the typically developing children for specific joint segments. The relationship between group and movement outcome (smoothness of movement) was mediated by inter-limb coordination variability. Therefore, the poor coordination and slower learning generally reported in children with DCD could be due to a slower or less optimal exploration of motor solutions.
-
Harris S, Wilmut K, Rathbone C, 'Anxiety, Confidence and Self-Concept in Adults with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 119 (2021)
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBackground: Previous research suggests that adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) report lower general wellbeing and higher general anxiety levels than typically developing (TD) adults.
Aims: To examine and explore relationships between anxiety and confidence (self-efficacy and resilience) generally and in a movement-specific context, along with self-concept among adults with DCD and TD adults.
Methods: 74 adults with diagnosed DCD, 26 adults with suspected DCD and 79 TD adults (18-60 years) completed an online questionnaire composed of a mixture of existent psychometric measures and novel scales.
Results: General and movement-specific anxiety, self-efficacy and general resilience were all poorer in adults with diagnosed and suspected DCD compared to TD adults. Higher resilience was related to higher self-efficacy and lower anxiety in adults with DCD. Individuals with suspected DCD for whom motor skills difficulties were an important aspect of their self-concept had lower movement-specific self-efficacy.
Conclusions: Interventions to improve the psychosocial wellbeing of adults with DCD should include a focus on lowering anxiety and building self-efficacy and resilience, with particular attention to movement-related domains.
Implications: This would facilitate the effective development of strategies to manage motor skills difficulties and their impact on everyday life for adults with DCD.
-
Meachon EJ, Meyer M, Wilmut K, Zemp M, Alpers GW, 'Evoked Potentials Differentiate Developmental Coordination Disorder from Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a Stop-Signal Task: A Pilot Study'
Frontiers in Psychology 15 (2021)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDevelopmental Coordination Disorder and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder are unique neurodevelopmental disorders with overlaps in executive functions and motor control. The conditions co-occur in up to 50% of cases, raising questions of the pathological mechanisms of DCD versus ADHD. Few studies have examined these overlaps in adults with DCD and/or ADHD. Therefore, to provide insights about executive functions and motor control between adults with DCD, ADHD, both conditions (DCD+ADHD), or typically developed controls, this study used a stop-signal task and parallel EEG measurement. We assessed executive performance via go accuracy and go reaction time, as well as motor response inhibition via stop signal reaction time. This was complemented with analysis of event-related potentials (ERPs). Based on existing investigations of adults with DCD or ADHD, we expected groups would (1) not differ in behavioral performance on stop and go trials, but (2) differences in ERPs, particularly in components N200 (index of cognitive control) and P300 (index of attention and inhibition) would be evident. The sample included N=50 adults with DCD (n=12), ADHD (n=9), DCD+ADHD (n=7), and control participants (n=22). We replicated that there were no between-group differences for behavioral-level executive performance and motor response inhibition. However, on a physiological level, ERP components N200 and P300 differed between groups, particularly during successful response inhibition. These ERPs reflect potential endophenotypic differences not evident in overt behavior of participants with ADHD and/or DCD. This suggests a disorder specific employment of inhibition or general executive functions in groups of adults with DCD, DCD+ADHD, ADHD, or control participants.
-
Wilmut K, Purcell C, 'Why are older adults more at risk as pedestrians? A systematic review'
Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 64 (8) (2021) pp.1269-1291
ISSN: 0018-7208 eISSN: 1547-8181AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARObjective: To explore factors which could explain why older adults are more at risk at the roadside.
Background: The physical and psychological health benefits of walking have been well-established, leading to the widespread promotion of walking amongst older adults. However, walking can result in an increased risk of injury as a pedestrian at the roadside, which is a greater risk for older adults who are over-represented in pedestrian casualty figures.
Method: Relevant databases were searched up to January 2020. All peer reviewed journals that presented data on healthy older adults and some aspect of road crossing or roadside behaviour were included. A total of 142 papers were assessed and 60 met the inclusion criteria.
Results: Identified papers could be grouped into three areas: crossing at a designated crossing place; crossing with no designated crossing place; perceptions or behaviours.
Conclusion: Multiple individual (attitudes, perceived behavioural control, walking time, time-to-arrival judgements, waiting endurance, cognitive ability), task (vehicle size, vehicle speed, traffic volume) and environmental (road layout, time of day, weather) constraints influence road crossing in older adulthood.
Application: Accessibility of designated crossing areas needs to be addressed by ensuring sufficient time to cross and non-restrictive waiting times. Signalised crossings need to be simplified and visibility increased. Where there is no designated crossing place a reduction in speed limit alongside the provision of pedestrian islands to provide ‘pause’ places are needed. Educational based programmes may also help ensure safety of older adults where there is no designated crossing place.
-
Wilmut K, Purcell C, 'The nature of the risk faced by pedestrians with neurodevelopmental disorders: A systematic review'
Accident Analysis & Prevention 149 (2020)
ISSN: 0001-4575 eISSN: 1879-2057AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPedestrians represent one of the most vulnerable road user groups worldwide. Children and adult pedestrians with neurodevelopmental disorders may be at greater risk due to deficits in a range of domains, such as attention, social communication, motor control and executive function. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (American Psychological Association, 2013), neurodevelopmental disorders include individuals with a diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorders, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Specific Learning Disorder, Motor Difficulties, Communication Disorders and Intellectual Disabilities. The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to explore existing literature relating to determine the nature of the risk faced by pedestrians with neurodevelopmental disorders. Relevant databases including Web of Science, PhysInfo and CINAHL were searched up to July 2019. All peer reviewed journals that presented data focusing on neurodevelopmental disorders and some aspect of road crossing or roadside behaviour that included a control or comparison group were included. A total of 149 abstracts were assessed and 17 met the inclusion criteria. The identified papers could be grouped into four areas: (1) rate of injury; (2) assessment of risk; (3) eye gaze and understanding of road layout and (4) gap choice. No papers exploring the risk factors at the roadside for individuals with Specific Learning Disorders or Communication Disorders were identified. Overall, the review provide evidence for an elevated risk of injury for individuals with ADHD at the roadside, potentially as a consequence of poor temporal gap choice, although there was evidence that this risk could be mediated by executive dysfunction rather than ADHD symptomology. Furthermore, poor temporal gap choice was found in children with DCD but it remains unclear as to whether this risk translates to the roadside. Finally, both children and adults with ASD and children with ID were found to demonstrate differences in behaviour / understanding at the roadside. In general, co-occurrence between neurodevelopmental disorders has been largely ignored in the current literature relating to pedestrian risk and future research could consider this along with executive functioning.
-
Harris S, Wilmut K, 'To Step or To Spring: The Influence of State Anxiety on Perceptual Judgments and Executed Action'
Experimental Brain Research 238 (2020) pp.843-849
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADAREmotional state and in particular anxiety has been shown to constrain perceptual judgement of action capabilities. However, whether anxiety also constrains actual behaviour is unknown. The current study therefore aimed to determine whether state anxiety constrained firstly perceptual judgements of action capabilities and secondly actual behaviour. To do this we asked participants to make perceptual judgements and perform action behaviours in relation to crossing ground-based apertures representing puddles. State anxiety was measured in 30 participants using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). The critical ratio of aperture-size relative to leg length at which participants’ behaviour choice would switch between a step and a spring was calculated. In a perceptual judgment task participants judged the ratio at which they would choose to switch. In a subsequent executed action task, the ratio at which they actually switched was measured. Perceptual critical ratio could be predicted via state anxiety and age, while action critical ratio was not predicted by either. Therefore, this study has demonstrated that state anxiety and age both constrain perceptual judgement of action capabilities, as shown in previous studies. However, this does not seem to result in a change in emergent behaviour. This highlights the importance of measuring emergent behaviour rather than inferring it from perceptual judgements even when they are couched in terms of action.
-
Wilmut K, Purcell C, 'The lived experience of crossing the road when you have Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD): the perspectives of parents of children with DCD and adults with DCD'
Frontiers in Psychology 11 (2020)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPedestrians are one of the most vulnerable groups at the roadside, furthermore, previous research has demonstrated perceptual-motor limitations in individuals with DCD which may put these individuals at even more at risk in the context of road crossing. However, it is unclear whether this is the lived experience of these individuals at the roadside. Furthermore, difficulties with road crossing and safety have been found in other neurodevelopmental disorders but the impact this might have on an individual with co-occurring difficulties is unknown. Therefore, we utilised a questionnaire to survey the lived experience of adults with DCD and parents of children with DCD with the specific objectives of describing behaviours exhibited by adults and children with DCD (the latter reported by parents) at the roadside and to determine the how these individuals perceive road crossing actions. For each of these we compared different co-occurrence groups. We also had one final objective which was not focussed on road crossing but more on the general perception of accidents and unrealistic optimism. Individuals with co-occurrences which have previously been linked to unsafe crossing behaviours (i.e. ADHD, ASD and LD) reported a greater regularity of dangerous looking behaviour (forgetting to look, running without looking) and visibility (crossing between cars, crossing when you can’t see), these adults and the parents of these children were seemingly aware of the risky nature of these behaviours. When asked ‘why’ crossing ability might be different, perceptual and motor difficulties alongside heightened awareness of risk and lowered awareness of risk were all cited by participants. Unrealistic optimism was not an explanation for the risky behaviour in adults with DCD and in fact, these adults demonstrated a clear understanding of the likelihood of accidents. The findings of this study suggest that road crossing is perceived to be more challenging for both children and adults with DCD and this needs to be taken into account when considering remediation for this group.
-
Wang S, Williams J, Wilmut K, 'Constraints on motor planning across the lifespan: Physical, cognitive and motor factors'
Psychology and Aging 35 (3) (2019) pp.421-433
ISSN: 0882-7974 eISSN: 1939-1498AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARSkilled adult movers plan for a comfortable end position even when this requires an uncomfortable start position (end-state-comfort effect). This ability declines in late adulthood and has been linked to age-related differences in cognitive functioning. Other factors, which may also drive difference in motor planning in later adulthood have not been systematically examined. These include perceptions of comfort and levels of motor imagery ability (one’s ability to mentally simulate action/predict the outcome of action). Therefore, this study investigated the constraints on movement planning across the life span, including executive functions, general motor ability, physical constraints to movement and motor imagery ability. One hundred and twenty-two participants aged 20–81 years completed an end-state-comfort task with increasing levels of complexity. Individuals’ executive functions, motor control, motor imagery ability, and perceived rotation span were also examined. Age-related decline was shown in planning for sequential movements but not in simple single-step movements. Motor planning demonstrated an age-related difference that was associated with an increasing number of constraints as age increased, and in older adults chronological age influenced the effect of each constraint on motor planning. Age-related difference in motor planning may reflect effective compensatory strategies in response to differing constraints in motor imagery ability, executive functions, perceived rotation span, and general speed and accuracy of movement as we age.
-
Bhoyroo R, Hands B, Wilmut K, Hyde C, Wigley A, 'Motor planning with and without motor imagery in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Acta Psychologica 199 (2019)
ISSN: 0001-6918AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARChildren with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) demonstrate inefficient motor planning ability with a tendency to opt for non-optimal planning strategies. Motor imagery can provide an insight to this planning inefficiency, as it may be a strategy for improving motor planning and thereby motor performance for those with DCD. In this study, we investigated the prevalence of end-state-comfort (ESC) and the minimal rotation strategy using a grip selection task in children with DCD with and without motor imagery instructions. Boys with (n = 14) and without DCD (n = 18) aged 7 – 12 years completed one, two and three colour sequences of a grip selection (octagon) task. Two conditions were examined; a Motor Planning (MP) condition requiring only the performance of the task and a Motor Imagery and Planning (MIP) condition, which included an instruction to imagine performing the movement before execution. For the MP condition, children with DCD ended fewer trials in ESC for the one (p=0.001) and two colour (p=0.002) sequences and used a minimal rotation strategy more often than those without DCD. For the MIP condition, the DCD group significantly increased their use of the ESC strategy for the one colour sequences (p=0.014) while those without DCD improved for the two colour (p=0.008) sequences. ESC level of the DCD group on the MIP condition was similar to those without DCD at baseline for all colour sequences. Motor imagery shows potential as a strategy for improving motor planning in children with DCD. Implications and limitations are discussed.
-
Wilmut K, Wang S, 'The "why" of reaching: second-order planning across the adult lifespan'
Psychology and Aging 34 (3) (2019) pp.431-440
ISSN: 0882-7974 eISSN: 1939-1498AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARSecond-order planning, planning which takes into account imminent and subsequent task demands, has been shown to be essential during everyday movement. For example, the kinematics of a ‘reach to an object’ action have been shown to be linked to the intended goal for the object (the prior intention). However, it is unclear whether this type of second-order planning for prior intention is preserved during aging, or indeed how this differs across the adult lifespan. Kinematics of a reach action preceding four prior intentions, place in a ‘tight’ hole, place in a ‘loose’ hole, throw or lift were measured in 122 aged from 20-81 years. The kinematics of the reach movement demonstrated that all participants tailored their reach movement to the prior intention, with the deceleration period of the reach discriminating across groups. The 20s and 30s group showed a different deceleration period during the reach for tight versus loose place prior intentions, this was not seen after 39 years of age and the 70+ group showed no discrimination across the deceleration period for the four prior intentions. When considering movement efficiency of the place actions we found it could be predicted by age and that this relationship was mediated by discrimination across the deceleration period. This study demonstrates that a clear difference is seen in the way in which second-order planning is used across the lifespan and that this has implications for movement efficiency.
-
Bhoyroo R, Hands B, Wilmut K, Hyde C, Wigley A, 'Investigating motor planning in children with DCD: Evidence from simple and complex grip-selection tasks'
Human Movement Science 61 (2018) pp.42-51
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractSeveral studies suggest that children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) may be able to plan simple movements as well as their peers, but experience increasing difficulties as the movements become complex. The present study aimed to clarify the nature of motor planning in DCD, including a putative deficit, by being the first to investigate motor planning using converging measures of simple and complex motor planning in a single sample of children with DCD. Boys aged between 8 – 12 years with (n = 10) and without DCD (n = 17) completed three commonly used ‘simple’ (bar grasping, sword, and bar transport tasks) measures and one ‘complex’ (octagon task) measure of end-state-comfort (ESC), a classic measurement of motor planning ability. To achieve ESC when manipulating an object, a person may choose to start with an uncomfortable grip in order to end the movement in a comfortable position. Results indicate that the participants with DCD planned for ESC as efficiently as their peers when performing the ‘simple’ measures of ESC but were significantly less likely to end their performances in ESC than those without DCD for the more ‘complex’ octagon task. Taken together, our data suggest that school-aged children with DCD may be able to plan simple movements as efficiently as their peers, but have more difficulty doing so for multi-movement or complex sequences. Based on the assumption that the efficiency of such motor planning is dependent on the integrity of internal modelling systems, we argue that our study provides indirect support for the internal modelling deficit hypothesis.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, Barnett AL, 'When an object appears unexpectedly: foot placement during obstacle circumvention in children and adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Experimental Brain Research 235 (10) (2017) pp.2947-2958
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARAdjustments to locomotion to avoid an obstacle require a change to the usual pattern of foot
placement, i.e. changes to step length and/or step width. Previous studies have demonstrated a
difficulty in individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) in controlling stability
while both stepping over and while circumventing an obstacle. In a previous study we have
considered the way in which individuals with DCD prepare for the possibility of an obstacle
appearing (Wilmut and Barnett, 2017). Using a parallel data set from this same task on the same
individuals the aim of the current study was to investigate the exact nature of changes in foot
placement during obstacle avoidance, as this was not clear from previous work. Children and adults aged from 7 to 34 years of age took part in the study. Forty-four met the criteria for a diagnosis of DCD and there were 44 typically developing (TD) age and gender matched controls. Participants walked at a comfortable pace down an 11m walkway; on 6 out of 36 trials a ‘gate’ closed across their pathway which required circumvention. These 6 ‘gate close’ trials were analysed for this study. The number and magnitude of step length and step width adjustments were similar across the DCD and TD groups, however, the younger children (7-11yrs) made a greater number of early adjustments compared to the older children and adults (12-34 years of age). In contrast the adults made a greater number of adjustments later in the movement compared to the children. In terms of foot placement adjustments a clear preference was seen across all participants to use adjustments which resulted in reducing step length, stepping away from the obstacle and a combination of these. Apart from subtle differences, the individuals with DCD make step placements to circumvent an obstacle in line with their peers. It is suggested that the choice of foot placement strategy in individuals with DCD, although in line with their peers, may not be optimal for their level of motor ability.
-
Wilmut K, 'Performance under varying constraints in Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD): Difficulties and compensations'
Current Developmental Disorders Reports 4 (2) (2017) pp.46-52
ISSN: 2196-2987AbstractPurpose of Review. Developmental coordination disorder is by very nature a disorder of movement and coordination. The constraints-based approach to motor control advocates the idea that the environment, the task and the individual can all constrain and promote movement/coordination. The purpose of this review is to describe factors which have been shown to explain the movement patterns in developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and discuss these in light of the constraints-based approach. Recent Findings. Recent findings, within the DCD field, considering the perception-action relationship, the control of movement, the role of vision and individual differences in DCD can all be considered under the constraints-based which allows us to consider all factors involved in movement. Summary. This paper has demonstrated the usefulness of the constraints-based approach in considering DCD and has also raised important questions regarding how we group and describe these individuals.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, Barnett AL, 'When an object appears unexpectedly: anticipatory movement and object circumvention in individuals with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Experimental Brain Research 235 (5) (2017) pp.1531-1540
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractObstacles often appear unexpectedly in our pathway and these require us to make adjustments to avoid collision. Previous research has demonstrated that healthy adults will make anticipatory adjustments to gait where they have been told there is the possibility of an obstacle appearing. One population that may find this type of anticipatory movement difficult is individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). The current study considered how individuals with and without DCD adjust to the possibility of an obstacle appearing which would require circumvention. Forty four individuals with DCD and 44 agematched controls (aged from 7-34 years of age) walked down an 11m walkway under three conditions. Initially they were told this was a clear pathway and nothing in the environment would change (1, no possibility of an obstacle, no obstacle). They then performed a series of trials in which a gate may (2, possibility of an obstacle, obstacle) or may not (3, possibility of an obstacle, no obstacle) partially obstruct their pathway. We found that all participants increased medio-lateral trunk acceleration when there was the possibility of an obstacle but before the obstacle appeared, in addition the typical adults and older children also increased step width. When describing circumvention we found that the younger children showed an increase in trunk velocity and acceleration in all three directions compared to older children and adults. We also found that the individuals with DCD adjusted their path sooner and deviated more than their peers. The degree of adjustment to step width in anticipation of an obstacle was related to later medio-lateral velocity and timing of the deviation. Therefore, the lack of ‘readying’ the system where there is the possibility of an obstacle appearing seen in the individuals with DCD and the younger typical children may explain the increased mediolateral velocity seen during circumvention.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Purcell C, Wilmut K, Wann J, 'The Use of Visually Guided Behaviour in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) when Crossing a Virtual Road'
Human Movement Science 53 (2017) pp.37-44
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractThe ability to safely cross a road is a perceptual-motor skill that involves coordination between a pedestrian’s perception of the approaching vehicles and their locomotive capability to execute the road crossing action. Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) is a chronic disorder that is characterised by significant motor difficulties that impact on daily living, including a reduced ability to perform visually guided actions. A total of 25 typically developing primary school aged children and 25 age- and gender-matched children with DCD were presented with a virtual desktop task that required them to select suitable temporal crossing gaps between vehicles a stream of traffic approaching at either 20 mph, 30 mph or 40 mph from the near-side (one-lane) or both near + far-sides (two-lane). A best-PEST staircase procedure was used to measure the temporal gaps that children accepted and the maximum likelihood value was taken after nine reversals as each participant’s threshold. Typically developing children accepted temporal gaps that were sufficient to execute a safe crossing for vehicles approaching at 20 mph and 30 mph, but insufficient for vehicles approaching at 40 mph. In contrast, children with DCD selected insufficient temporal crossing gaps across all approach speeds, which if translated to the roadside would have resulted in collision. These findings add to our understanding of the difficulties that children with DCD appear to have with visually guided behaviour and suggest the potential impact on one aspect of daily functioning that could have significant consequences.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, Du W, Barnett AL, 'When an object appears unexpectedly: object circumvention in adults'
Journal of Motor Behavior 49 (6) (2016) pp.629-639
ISSN: 0022-2895AbstractObstacles often appear unexpectedly in our pathway and these require us to make immediate adjustments. Despite how regularly we encounter such situations only few studies have considered how we adjust to unexpected obstacles in the pathway which require us to walk around them. The current study considered how adults adjust to the possibility of an obstacle appearing and then also how foot placement is adjusted to circumvent an obstacle. Fifteen healthy adults walked down an 11m walkway, initially they were told this was a clear pathway and nothing in the environment would change (no gate), they then performed a series of trials in which a gate may (gate close) or may not (gate open) partially obstruct their pathway. We found that medio-lateral trunk velocity and acceleration was significantly increased when there was the possibility of an obstacle but before the obstacle appeared. This demonstrates an adaptive walking strategy which seems to enable healthy young adults to successfully circumvent obstacles. We also categorised foot placement adjustments and found that adults favoured making shorter and wider steps away from the obstacle. We suggest this combination of adjustments allows participants to maintain stability whilst successfully circumventing the obstacle.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, Gentle J, Barnett AL, 'Gait symmetry in individuals with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 60 (2016) pp.107-114
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractBackground: Symmetry between the left and right side of the body during locomotion is key in a coordinated gait cycle and is also thought to be important in terms of efficiency. Although previous studies have identified aspects of the gait cycle which are atypical in children and adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD), studies have not considered whether this could be explained by asymmetrical gait.Published here Open Access on RADARMethod and procedure: The current study included 62 participants with and 62 without DCD (aged 7-34 years). Participants were asked to walk continuously for 1 minute up and down a walkway while movement was captured using an optical tracking system. Measures of step length and step time were taken for both the right and the left leg and symmetry ratios were calculated.
Results: The DCD group showed significantly higher symmetry ratios for both measures compared to the typically developing (TD) group, with approximately a third of DCD participants falling outside the normative range for symmetry. Furthermore, a relationship was found between movement variability and degree of asymmetry.
Conclusions: These findings demonstrate an asymmetry in the gait of individuals with DCD which, despite improving with age, does not reach the same level as that shown by TD individuals.
-
Wilmut K, Du W, Barnett A, 'Navigating through apertures: perceptual judgements and actions of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Developmental Science 20 (6) (2016)
ISSN: 1363-755XAbstractPassing through a narrow gap/aperture involves a perceptual judgement regarding the size of the gap and an action to pass through. Children with DCD are known to have difficulties with perceptual judgments in near space but whether this extends to far space is unknown. Furthermore, in a recent study it was found that adults with DCD do not scale movements when walking through an aperture in the same way as their peers. The current study, therefore, considered perceptual judgments and motor behaviour of children with DCD while looking at or walking through apertures. Twenty-nine children with DCD and 29 typically developing (TD) children took part. In Experiment 1, participants completed a perceptual task, where they made passability judgements. Children with DCD showed a significantly smaller critical ratio (aperture size at which a participant first rotates the shoulders to pass through) compared to their TD peers. In Experiment 2, participants completed an action task where they walked through the same apertures. Children with DCD showed a significantly larger critical ratio than TD peers when body size alone was accounted for. Taken together these results suggest that perception within a static context is different from that within a dynamic context for children with DCD. However, despite this difference we have demonstrated a clear relationship between perception and action in children with DCD.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Gentle J, Barnett AL, Wilmut K, 'Adaptations to walking on an uneven terrain for individuals with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Human Movement Science 49 (2016) pp.346-353
ISSN: 0167-9457 eISSN: 1872-7646AbstractGiven the importance of walking in everyday life, understanding why this is challenging for some populations is particularly important. Studies focusing on gait patterns of individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) have shown that whilst increased variability is characteristic of walking patterns for this group, differences in spatio-temporal gait variables seem only to arise when task demands increase. However, these differences occur under rather artificial conditions, for example using a treadmill. The aim of this study, therefore was to examine the step characteristics of individuals with and without DCD whilst walking along an irregular terrain. Thirty-five individuals with DCD aged 8-32 years and 35 age and gender-matched controls participated in this study. Participants were divided into 3 age groups; 8-12years (n = 12), 13-17years (n =12) and 18-32years (n=11). Participants walked up and down a 6m walkway for two minutes on two terrains: level and irregular. VICON 3D motion analysis was used to extract measures of foot placement, velocity and angle of the head and trunk. Results showed that both groups adapted their gait to negotiate the irregular terrain, but the DCD group were more affected than their TD peers; walking significantly slower with shorter, wider steps and inclining their head more towards the ground. This suggests an adaptive approach used by individuals with DCD to preserve stability and increase visual sampling whilst negotiating an irregular terrain.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Prunty M, Barnett AL, Wilmut, K, Plumb M, 'Visual perceptual and handwriting skills in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Human Movement Science 49 (2016) pp.54-65
ISSN: 0167-9457 eISSN: 1872-7646AbstractObjectivePublished here Open Access on RADARChildren with Developmental Coordination Disorder demonstrate a lack of automaticity in handwriting as measured by pauses during writing. Deficits in visual perception have been proposed in the literature as underlying mechanisms of handwriting difficulties in children with DCD. The aim of this study was to examine whether correlations exist between measures of visual perception and visual motor integration with measures of the handwriting product and process in children with DCD.
Method
The performance of twenty-eight 8–14 year-old children who met the DSM-5 criteria for DCD was compared with 28 typically developing (TD) age and gender-matched controls. The children completed the Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration (VMI) and the Test of Visual Perceptual Skills (TVPS). Group comparisons were made, correlations were conducted between the visual perceptual measures and handwriting measures and the sensitivity and specificity examined.
Results
The DCD group performed below the TD group on the VMI and TVPS. There were no significant correlations between the VMI or TVPS and any of the handwriting measures in the DCD group. In addition, both tests demonstrated low sensitivity.
Conclusion
Clinicians should execute caution in using visual perceptual measures to inform them about handwriting skill in children with DCD.
-
Prunty M, Barnett A, Wilmut K, Plumb M, 'The Impact of Handwriting Difficulties on Compositional Quality in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
British Journal of Occupational Therapy 79 (10) (2016) pp.591-597
ISSN: 0308-0226 eISSN: 1477-6006AbstractIntroduction: There is substantial evidence to support the relationship between transcription skills (handwriting and spelling) and compositional quality. For children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) handwriting can be particularly challenging. While recent research has aimed to investigate their handwriting difficulties in more detail, the impact of transcription on their compositional quality has not previously been examined. The aim of this exploratory study was to examine compositional quality in children with DCD and to ascertain whether their transcription skills influence writing quality.Published here Open Access on RADAR
Method: Twenty eight children with DCD participated in the study, with 28 typically developing (TD) age and gender matched controls. The children completed the ‘free writing’ task from the Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting (DASH), which was evaluated for compositional quality using the Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions (WOLD).
Results: The children with DCD performed significantly below their TD peers on 5 of the 6 WOLD items. They also had a higher percentage of misspelled words. Regression analyses indicated that the number of words produced per minute and the percentage of misspelled words explained 55% of the variance for compositional quality.
Conclusion: The handwriting difficulties so commonly reported in children with DCD have wider repercussions for the quality of written composition.
-
Wilmut K, Du W, Barnett AL, 'Gait patterns in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Experimental Brain Research 234 (6) (2016) pp.1747-1755
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrevious research has shown that adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) show increased variability of foot placement measures and movement of the centre of mass (CoM) while walking. The current study considered the gait patterns of young and older children with DCD. Fourteen young children with DCD (7-12 years), 15 older children with DCD (12-17 years) and 29 age and gender matched typically developing children took part. Children were asked to walk up and down a flat 10m long pathway for one minute while the movement of their feet and trunk was recorded using motion analysis. The gait pattern of children with DCD was characterised by wider steps, elevated variability in the time spent in double support and stride time and greater medio-lateral velocity and acceleration compared to their peers. An elevated variability in medio-lateral acceleration was also seen in the young but not the older children with DCD. In addition, the young children showed a greater variability in velocity and acceleration in all three directions compared to the older children. The data suggest that the high incidence of trips and falls seen in children with DCD may be due to differences in the control of the centre of mass.
-
Fuelscher I, Williams J, Wilmut K, Enticott PG, Hyde C, 'Modeling the Maturation of Grip Selection Planning and Action Representation: Insights from Typical and Atypical Motor Development'
Frontiers in Psychology 7 (2016) pp.1-14
ISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARWe investigated the purported association between developmental changes in grip selection planning and improvements in an individual’s capacity to represent action at an internal level (i.e., motor imagery). Participants were groups of healthy children aged 6-7 years and 8-12 years respectively, while a group of adolescents (13-17 years) and adults (18-34 years) allowed for consideration of childhood development in the broader context of motor maturation. A group of children aged 8-12 years with probable DCD (pDCD) was included as a reference group for atypical motor development. Participants’ proficiency to generate and/or engage internal action representations was inferred from performance on the hand rotation task, a well-validated measure of motor imagery. A grip selection task designed to elicit the end-state comfort (ESC) effect provided a window into the integrity of grip selection planning. Consistent with earlier accounts, the efficiency of grip selection planning followed a non-linear developmental progression in neurotypical individuals. As expected, analysis confirmed that these developmental improvements were predicted by an increased capacity to generate and/or engage internal action representations. The profile of this association remained stable throughout the (typical) developmental spectrum. These findings are consistent with computational accounts of action planning that argue that internal action representations are associated with the expression and development of grip selection planning across typical development. However, no such association was found for our sample of children with pDCD, suggesting that individuals with atypical motor skill may adopt an alternative, sub-optimal strategy to plan their grip selection compared to their same-age control peers.
-
Du W, Barnett AL, Wilmut K, 'The role of movement variability and action experience in the perceptual judgement of passability'
Journal of Motor Learning and Development 4 (2) (2016) pp.307-323
ISSN: 2325-3193AbstractPerception and action are tightly coupled and previous studies have demonstrated that action experience can improve perceptual judgement. We investigated whether this improvement in perceptual judgement could be attributed to knowledge regarding movement variability being gained during action experience. Fifteen adults made perceptual judgments regarding the passability of a series of aperture sizes. These judgements were made both before and after walking through the same set of apertures (action experience). When considering the group as a whole perceptual judgement did not change after action experience. However, when splitting the group into those with low and high pre-action perceptual judgements, only those with low perceptual judgements showed an improvement in perceptual judgement following action experience and this could in part be explained by movement variability during the approach. These data demonstrate that action informs perception and that this allows adults to account for movement variability when making perceptual judgements regarding action capabilities.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Du W, Wilmut K, Barnett AL, 'Level walking in adults with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder: an analysis of movement variability'
Human Movement Science 43 (2015) pp.9-14
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARSeveral studies have shown that Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) is a condition that continues beyond childhood. Although adults with DCD report difficulties with dynamic balance, as well as frequent tripping and bumping into objects, there have been no specific studies on walking in this population. Some previous work has focused on walking in children with DCD but variation in the tasks and measures used has led to inconsistent findings. The aim of the current study therefore was to examine the characteristics of level walking in adults with and without DCD. Fifteen adults with DCD and 15 typically developing (TD) controls walked barefoot at a natural pace up and down an 11 m walkway for one minute. Foot placement measures and velocity and acceleration of the body were recorded, as well as measures of movement variability. The adults with DCD showed similar gait patterns to the TD group in terms of step length, step width, double support time and stride time. The DCD group also showed similar velocity and acceleration to the TD group in the medio-lateral, anterior-posterior and vertical direction. However, the DCD group exhibited greater variability in all foot placement and some body movement measures. The finding that adults with DCD have a reduced ability to produce consistent movement patterns is discussed in relation to postural control limitations and compared to variability of walking measures found in elderly populations.
-
Wilmut K, Du W, Barnett AL, 'How do I fit through that gap? : navigation through apertures in adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
PLoS ONE 10 (4) (2015)
ISSN: 1932-6203AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDuring everyday life we move around busy environments and encounter a range of obstacles,
such as a narrow aperture forcing us to rotate our shoulders in order to pass through.
In typically developing individuals the decision to rotate the shoulders is body scaled and
this movement adaptation is temporally and spatially tailored to the size of the aperture.
This is done effortlessly although it actually involves many complex skills. For individuals
with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) moving in a busy environment and negotiating
obstacles presents a real challenge which can negatively impact on safety and participation
in motor activities in everyday life. However, we have a limited understanding of the
nature of the difficulties encountered. Therefore, this current study considered how adults
with DCD make action judgements and movement adaptations while navigating apertures.
Fifteen adults with DCD and 15 typically developing (TD) controls passed through a series
of aperture sizes which were scaled to body size (0.9-2.1 times shoulder width). Spatial and
temporal characteristics of movement were collected over the approach phase and while
crossing the aperture. The decision to rotate the shoulders was not scaled in the same way
for the two groups, with the adults with DCD showing a greater propensity to turn for larger
apertures compared to the TD adults when body size alone was accounted for. However,
when accounting for degree of lateral trunk movement and variability on the approach, we
no longer saw differences between the two groups. In terms of the movement adaptations,
the adults with DCD approached an aperture differently when a shoulder rotation was required
and then adapted their movement sooner compared to their typical peers. These results
point towards an adaptive strategy in adults with DCD which allows them to account
for their movement difficulties and avoid collision.
-
Prunty M, Barnett AL, Wilmut K, Plumb MS, 'An examination of writing pauses in the handwriting of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 35 (11) (2014) pp.2894-2905
ISSN: 0891-4222AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDifficulties with handwriting are reported as one of the main reasons for the referral of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) to healthcare professionals. In a recent study we found that children with DCD produced less text than their typically developing (TD) peers and paused for 60% of a free-writing task. However, little is known about the nature of the pausing; whether they are long pauses possibly due to higher level processes of text generation or fatigue, or shorter pauses related to the movements between letters. This gap in the knowledge-base creates barriers to understanding the handwriting difficulties in children with DCD. The aim of this study was to characterise the pauses observed in the handwriting of English children with and without DCD. Twenty-eight 8–14 year-old children with a diagnosis of DCD participated in the study, with 28 TD age and gender matched controls. Participants completed the 10 min free-writing task from the Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting (DASH) on a digitising writing tablet. The total overall percentage of pausing during the task was categorised into four pause time-frames, each derived from the literature on writing (250 ms to 2 s; 2–4 s; 4–10 s and >10 s). In addition, the location of the pauses was coded (within word/between word) to examine where the breakdown in the writing process occurred. The results indicated that the main group difference was driven by more pauses above 10 s in the DCD group. In addition, the DCD group paused more within words compared to TD peers, indicating a lack of automaticity in their handwriting. These findings may support the provision of additional time for children with DCD in written examinations. More importantly, they emphasise the need for intervention in children with DCD to promote the acquisition of efficient handwriting skill.
-
Wilmut K, Byrne M, 'Grip selection for sequential movements in children and adults with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Human Movement Science 36 (2014) pp.272-284
ISSN: 0167-9457 eISSN: 1872-7646AbstractPublished hereWhen generating a movement adults favor grasps which start the body in an uncomfortable position if they end in a comfortable position (the end-state-comfort effect). In contrast, children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) select grasps which require little initial hand rotation even though they result in an uncomfortable end position. The current study considered grip selection of individuals with DCD when asked to make simple one step movements and when making more complex multi-staged movements. Adults with DCD (N = 17, mean age 24:09, SD age = 52 months) and children with DCD (N = 20, mean age 9:00, SD age = 20 months) and age and gender matched controls rotated a disc so an arrow pointed toward a specific target(s). Task complexity was increased by increasing the number of targets from 1 to 3. Planning for end-state-comfort was seen in all groups albeit to a lesser extent in children versus adults. The children with DCD showed fewer grips for end-state-comfort compared to their peers and this was explained by a propensity to select minimal initial rotation grasps. This result was mirrored in adults with DCD but only for the longest movement sequence. These results suggest some changes in ability from childhood to adulthood in individuals with DCD.
-
Wilmut K, Byrne M, 'Influences of grasp selection in typically developing children'
Acta Psychologica 148 (2014) pp.181-187
ISSN: 0001-6918AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARWhen reaching towards an object, adults favour grasps which, following the intended action, end in a comfortable position even when this requires them to start in an uncomfortable position (the end-state-comfort effect). However, this strategy is not consistently used by children who instead seem to favour a minimal pre-contact rotation of the hand, even when this results in an uncomfortable end position. In terms of multiple movements, the strategies used for grip selection are unclear; adults may still grasp for end-state-comfort given their propensity to plan to the end of a movement; however, children who are less able to concatenate movement may tend to start-state-comfort movements. The current study considered grip selection in children ranging from 4 to 12 years and in a group of adults. Participants were asked to rotate a disc so that an arrow pointed towards a specific target(s), the number of sequences in a movement was increased from one to three. Planning for end-state-comfort was seen in all participants and a clear developmental trajectory was identified whereby the relative comfort of an end position could be directly predicted by age in months. Adults and 10–12-year-olds favoured an end-state-comfort strategy whereas the younger children gave equal weighting to end-state-comfort, start-state-comfort and no initial rotation strategies. All groups were able to end a movement comfortably when it was composed of three steps; however, the proportion of movements relying on an end-state-comfort strategy decreased as sequence length increase whereas the proportion of start-state-comfort and no initial rotation strategies increased. The current data support the concept that a mechanism for planning grasps may be based on motor experience.
-
Barnett A, Dawes H, Wilmut K, 'Constraints and facilitators to participation in physical activity in teenagers with DCD: an exploratory interview study'
Child: Care, Health and Development 39 (3) (2013) pp.393-403
ISSN: 0305-1862 eISSN: 1365-2214AbstractBackground Despite their movement difficulties, youngsters with Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) generally have sufficient capability for physical activity. However, they tend to be less physically active and less physically fit than their well co-ordinated age peers. The aim of this study was to use qualitative research methods to understand which factors constrain and facilitate participation in physical activity in teenagers with DCD, in order to help inform future health promotion programmes.Published here
Method Semi-structured interviews were conducted with eight teenagers with DCD (aged 13-15) and their parents. The interviews focused on how much physical activity was typically undertaken by the child and the perceived constraints and facilitators to being physically active. Interviews were transcribed and subjected to categorical-content analysis.Results Half of the children and all but one of the parents reported that the children did little physical activity. Although most children disliked competitive team games, they reported many physical activities that they did enjoy and they reported wanting to be more physically active. Perceived internal constraints to participation included poor motor skill, lack of motivation and reports of fatiguing easily. Perceived external constraints included difficulty travelling to activities, negative comments from peers and teachers' lack of understanding of DCD.Conclusions Reports of low levels of physical activity support previous literature and are a cause for concern for this group. The teenagers expressed the desire to be more active, yet the interviews revealed both personal and environmental constraints to engagement in physical activity. It is clear that these factors interact in a dynamic way and that teachers, schools and communities play an important role in creating a motivational environment for youngsters with DCD to engage in physical activity and learn to maintain an active lifestyle as they move into adulthood. -
Hyde C, Wilmut K, Fuelscher I, Williams J, 'Does Implicit Motor Imagery Ability Predict Reaching Correction Efficiency? a Test of Recent Models of Human Motor Control'
Journal of Motor Behavior 45 (2013) pp.259-269
ISSN: 0022-2895 eISSN: 1940-1027AbstractPublished hereNeurocomputational models of reaching indicate that efficient purposive correction of movement midflight (e.g., online control) depends on one's ability to generate and monitor an accurate internal (neural) movement representation. In the first study to test this empirically, the authors investigated the relationship between healthy young adults' implicit motor imagery performance and their capacity to correct their reaching trajectory. As expected, after controlling for general reaching speed, hierarchical regression demonstrated that imagery ability was a significant predictor of hand correction speed; that is, faster and more accurate imagery performance associated with faster corrections to reaching following target displacement at movement onset. They argue that these findings provide preliminary support for the view that a link exists between an individual's ability to represent movement mentally and correct movement online efficiently. -
Barnett A, Plumb M, Wilmut K, 'Handwriting Speed in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder: Are They Really Slower?'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 34 (2013) pp.2927-2936
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractHandwriting difficulties are often included in descriptions of Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). They are cited as the most common reason for referral to health professionals following parent and teacher concerns about slow and untidy writing. The aim of this study was to compare handwriting performance in English children with and without DCD across a range of writing tasks, to gain a better understanding of the nature of 'slowness' so commonly reported. Twenty-eight 8-14 year-old children with a diagnosis of DCD participated in the study, with 28 typically developing age and gender matched controls. Participants completed the four handwriting tasks from the Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting (DASH) and wrote their own name; all on a digitising writing tablet. The number of words written, speed of pen movements and the time spent pausing during the tasks were calculated. The findings confirmed what many professionals report, that children with DCD produce less text than their peers. However, this was not due to slow movement execution, but rather a higher percentage of time spent pausing. Discussion centres on the understanding of the pausing phenomenon in children with DCD and areas for further research.Published here -
Wilmut K, Byrne M, Barnett A L, 'Reaching to throw compared to reaching to place: a comparison across individuals with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 34 (1) (2013) pp.174-182
ISSN: 0891-4222 eISSN: 1873-3379AbstractPublished hereWhen picking up an object, adults show a longer deceleration phase when the onward action has a greater precision requirement. Tailoring action in this way is thought to need forward modelling in order to predict the consequences of movement. Some evidence suggests that young children also tailor reaching in this way; however, how this skill develops in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) is unknown. The current study compared the kinematics of reaching to an object when the onward intention was: to place the object on a target (either with high or low precision requirements), to throw the object or to lift the object vertically. Movements of both adults (N=18) and children (N=24) with DCD and their age-matched controls were recorded. The typically developing adults discriminated across all action types, the adults with DCD and the typically developing children only across the actions to place and throw and the children with DCD only between the actions to lift and throw. The results demonstrate developmental progression towards fine tuning the planning of reaching in relation to onward intentions. Both adults and children with DCD are able to plan movement using inverse models but this skill is not yet fully developed in early adulthood.
-
Wilmut K, Barnett A L, 'To Throw Or to Place: Does Onward Intention Affect How a Child Reaches for An Object?'
Experimental Brain Research 226 (2013) pp.421-429
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPicking up an object is a seemingly simple and isolated task; however, research has demonstrated that adults plan a reach-to-grasp movement on the basis of forthcoming actions. For example a greater deceleration period is seen in an initial reach movement which precedes a place movement compared to a throw movement. This task-specific or second-order motor planning is also seen in infants and toddlers; however, the developmental progression is unclear. Reach-to-grasp movements of 48 children, split into four age groups (4–5, 6–7, 8–9 and 10–11 years) were recorded. These movements preceded a tight place, a loose place or a throw action. All the children showed some degree of tailoring kinematics to the onward action. In the 4–5 year-old group, this was demonstrated by a longer movement duration in the place actions compared to the throw action. In the older children the proportion of time spent decelerating increased as the precision requirements of the task increased. These results demonstrate that all children are able to use second-order planning to integrate onward task demands into their movements. The capacity for this increases with age but is not fully mature at 11 years. These developmental effects may be explained by the relative weighting of costs involved in tailoring a reach action compared to the benefits of producing a more efficient onward action.Published here -
Purcell C, Wann J, Wilmut K, Poulter D, 'Reduced looming sensitivity in primary school children with Developmental Co-ordination Disorder'
Developmental Science 15 (3) (2012) pp.299-306
ISSN: 1363-755X eISSN: 1467-7687AbstractPublished hereAlmost all locomotor animals are sensitive to optical expansion (visual looming) and for most animals this sensitivity is evident very early in their development. In humans there is evidence that responses to looming stimuli begin in the first 6 weeks of life, but here we demonstrate that as children become independent their perceptual acuity needs to be 50 to 100 times better than has been demonstrated in infants in order to be skilful at collision avoidance at a roadside. We have recently established that sensitivity to the detection of visual looming in 6- to 11-year-old children is significantly below that of adults (Wann, Poulter & Purcell, 2011). Here, using comparable methods, we explore looming detection sensitivity in children with Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD), who show broad patterns of impairment in visuo-motor control. We presented visual simulations of approaching vehicles, scaled to represent different approach rates, to children with DCD aged between 6 and 11 years (n = 11) and typically developing age and gender matched controls (n = 11). Looming detection thresholds were measured under foveal and perifoveal viewing conditions, for isotropic expansion and isotropic expansion with simulated viewpoint motion. Our results show that there are situations in which children with DCD may fail to detect vehicles approaching at speeds in excess of 22 km/h, suggesting a developmental immaturity in looming sensitivity. This provides one of the first clear demonstrations of low-level motion processing deficits in children with DCD. The decrement observed may give rise to potential errors in the road crossing behaviour of these children, whereby approaching vehicles could be perceived as stationary. These findings further contribute towards understanding the adverse statistic that children under 9 years of age are four times more likely than adults to be involved in a road accident as a pedestrian.
-
Wilmut K, Barnett A L, 'Locomotor behaviour of children while navigating through apertures'
Experimental Brain Research 210 (2) (2011) pp.185-194
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished hereDuring everyday locomotion, we encounter a range of obstacles requiring specific motor responses; a narrow aperture which forces us to rotate our shoulders in order to pass through is one example. In adults, the decision to rotate their shoulders is body scaled (Warren and Whang in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:371-383, 1987), and the movement through is temporally and spatially tailored to the aperture size (Higuchi et al. in Exp Brain Res 175:50-59, 2006; Wilmut and Barnett in Hum Mov Sci 29:289-298, 2010). The aim of the current study was to determine how 8-to 10-year-old children make action judgements and movement adaptations while passing through a series of five aperture sizes which were scaled to body size (0.9, 1.1, 1.3, 1.5 and 1.7 times shoulder width). Spatial and temporal characteristics of movement speed and shoulder rotation were collected over the initial approach phase and while crossing the doorway threshold. In terms of making action judgements, results suggest that the decision to rotate the shoulders is not scaled in the same way as adults, with children showing a critical ratio of 1.61. Shoulder angle at the door could be predicted, for larger aperture ratios, by both shoulder angle variability and lateral trunk variability. This finding supports the dynamical scaling model (Snapp-Childs and Bingham in Exp Brain Res 198:527-533, 2009). In terms of movement adaptations, we have shown that children, like adults, spatially and temporally tailor their movements to aperture size.
-
Purcell C, Wann J P, Wilmut K, Poulter D, 'Roadside judgements in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (4) (2011) pp.1283-1292
ISSN: 0891-4222AbstractPublished hereAs pedestrians, the perceptual ability to accurately judge the relative rate of approaching vehicles and select a suitable crossing gap requires sensitivity to looming. It also requires that crossing judgments are synchronized with motoric capabilities. Previous research has suggested that children with Developmental Co-ordination Disorder (DCD) may have deficits in visual processing, specifically in detecting visual motion. It is possible, therefore that this population are at greater risk at the roadside. In a series of motion prediction tasks, several component roadside skills were assessed in 15 children with DCD, or at risk of DCD, aged between 6 and 11 years along with 15 typically developing age and gender matched controls. First, threshold errors for relative approach rate judgments (looming) were measured when vehicle size (car or truck) varied. Second, thresholds for crossing gap selection were measured for vehicle approach speeds of 32, 48, 64 and 80km/h (20-50mph). These were related to the walking speeds of children of different ages and profiles. We found that children with DCD showed a deficit in making relative approach rate judgments, using looming, which suggests they may not discern that a vehicle is travelling faster than the urban speed limit. Children with DCD also left considerably longer temporal crossing gaps than controls perhaps reflecting a lack of confidence in their ability, these preferred gaps were over twice the average inter-car gaps that occurred on roads around their school. Our findings raise a number of issues concerning children with DCD and their competence and potential limitations as pedestrians.
-
Wilmut K, Barnett A, 'Locomotor adjustments when navigating through apertures'
Human Movement Science 29 (2) (2010) pp.289-298
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractDuring everyday locomotion we encounter a range of obstacles which require specific motor responses, for example a narrow aperture forces us to rotate the shoulders to pass through. Research has demonstrated that the decision to rotate the shoulders is body scaled (Warren & Whang, 1987) and that the visuo-motor system generates a rotation proportional to aperture size (Higuchi, Cinelli, Greig, & Patla, 2006). The current study considered how shoulder angle and movement speed are tailored to aperture size in nine adults. Aperture sizes were classified into shoulder/aperture ratios (SA ratio), including two for which participants had to rotate (0.9/1.1) and two for which participants could pass freely (1.5/1.7). During the initial approach phase (first 3 s), shoulder rotation and movement speed were invariant across SA ratio. Later in the movement, angle of shoulder rotation and the magnitude and timing of the reduction in speed were proportional to SA ratio. The timing of the reduction in speed was progressively later in the movement as SA ratio increased, suggesting early adjustments of movement, such as the timing of the reduction in speed are tightly tuned to the ratio between aperture size and shoulder width, even when no later body adjustments are needed.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut K, 'Selection and assessment of children with developmental coordination disorder'
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 52 (3) (2010)
ISSN: 0012-1622 eISSN: 1469-8749AbstractPublished here -
Ho A, Wilmut K, 'Speech and oro-motor function in children with developmental coordination disorder: a pilot study'
Human Movement Science 29 (4) (2010) pp.605-614
ISSN: 0167-9457AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThe protracted maturation and development of speech articulation underlies the complexity of the skill, and suggests it may be an area susceptible to a general deficit in motor control. Recent research suggests a high co-occurrence between Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) and disordered speech production. Despite this there has been no systematic investigation of speech motor control in children with DCD. We conducted a pilot study which looked at speech motor control in a group of children with DCD (N = 5) and a group typically developing (TD) children (N = 5). Movements of the upper and lower lip were recorded during non-verbal movements, single words, syllable sequences, and sentence repetition. In the baseline conditions (normal talking speed or an isolated utterance) children with DCD demonstrated a typical pattern of movement, albeit a slower and shorter movement. In contrast, when task complexity was increased the children with DCD showed an atypical pattern of movement. It was concluded that children with DCD demonstrate inferior motor control for complex speech gestures, suggesting that the motor deficit in DCD may indeed be a more generalized phenomenon affecting the speech motor system.
-
Wilmut K, Wann J, 'The use of predictive information is impaired in the actions of children and young adults with developmental coordination disorder'
Experimental Brain Research 191 (4) (2008) pp.403-418
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThe need for a movement response may often be preceded by some advance information regarding direction or extent. We examined the ability of individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) to organise a movement in response to advance information. Pre-cues were presented and varied in the extent to which they indicated the response target. Both eye movement latencies and hand movements were measured. In the absence of pre-cues, individuals with DCD were as fast in initial hand movements as the typically developing (TD) participants, but were less efficient at correcting initial directional errors. A major difference was seen in the degree to which each group could use advance pre-cue information. TD participants were able to use pre-cue information to refine their actions. For the individuals with DCD this was only effective if there was no ambiguity in the advance cue and they had particular difficulty in using predictive motion cues. There were no differences in the speed of gaze responses which excluded an explanation relating to the dynamic allocation of attention. Individuals with DCD continued to rely on the slower strategy of fixating the target prior to initiating a hand movement, rather than using advance information to set initial movement parameters.
-
Wilmut K, Wann J, 'The Use of Prescanning in the Parameterization of Sequential Pointing and Reaching Movements'
Journal of Motor Behavior 40 (6) (2008) pp.558-567
ISSN: 0022-2895 eISSN: 1940-1027AbstractThe accuracy of reaching movements improves when active gaze can be used to fixate on targets. The advantage of free gaze has been attributed to the use of ocular proprioception or efference signals for online control. The time course of this process, however, is not established, and it is unclear how far in advance gaze can move and still be used to parameterize subsequent movements. In this experiment, the authors considered the advantage of prescanning targets for both pointing and reaching movements. The authors manipulated the visual information and examined the extent to which prescanning of targets could compensate for a reduction in online visual feedback. In comparison with a conventional reaching/pointing condition, the error in pointing was reduced, the eye-hand lead decreased, and both the hand-closure time and the size of the maximum grip aperture in reaching were modulated when prescanning was allowed. These results indicate that briefly prescanning multiple targets just prior to the movement allows the refinement of subsequent hand movements that yields an improvement in accuracy. This study therefore provides additional evidence that the coordinate information arising from efference or ocular-proprioceptive signals can, for a limited period, be buffered and later used to generate a sequence of movements.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Wilmut, K, Brown, J, Wann JP, 'Attention Disengagement in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Disability and Rehabilitation 29 (1) (2007) pp.47-55
ISSN: 0963-8288 eISSN: 1464-5165AbstractPublished herePURPOSE:
Previous research has shown that children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) have poorly developed strategies for allocating attention. This study examines the allocation of attention and integration of visuo-spatial and motor systems in children with DCD in a motor (look+hit condition) and a motor-free (look condition) task. METHOD:
Three groups of control children were used to compare the performance of a group of children with DCD. Children were seated in front of a central fixation point and six peripheral targets, and were asked to look at or hit targets when illuminated. Saccade/hand movement latencies were measured on gap trials (gap between fixation offset and target onset) and overlap trials (fixation offset and target onset overlapped). RESULTS:
DCD children were not slower than controls to disengage attention during the look condition. However, during the look+hit condition the DCD children showed a prolonged disengagement period, which was also seen in younger control children. CONCLUSIONS:
The results suggest that DCD children may have deficits in the allocation of attention for action, in both the speed of onset of a movement and the accuracy of the movement. It is concluded that attention disengagement may contribute to problems of visuo-motor integration in DCD. -
Farran E, Wilmut K, 'Texture segmentation in Williams Syndrome'
Neuropsychologia 45 (5) (2007) pp.1009-1018
ISSN: 0028-3932AbstractPublished hereWilliams syndrome (WS) is a developmental disorder in which visuo-spatial cognition is poor relative to verbal ability. At the level of visuo-spatial perception, individuals with WS can perceive both the local and global aspects of an image. However, the manner in which local elements are integrated into a global whole is atypical, with relative strengths in integration by luminance, closure, and alignment compared to shape, orientation and proximity. The present study investigated the manner in which global images are segmented into local parts. Segmentation by seven gestalt principles was investigated: proximity, shape, luminance, orientation, closure, size (and alignment: Experiment 1 only). Participants were presented with uniform texture squares and asked to detect the presence of a discrepant patch (Experiment 1) or to identify the form of a discrepant patch as a capital E or H (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, the pattern and level of performance of the WS group did not differ from that of typically developing controls, and was commensurate with the general level of non-verbal ability observed in WS. These results were replicated in Experiment 2, with the exception of segmentation by proximity, where individuals with WS demonstrated superior performance relative to the remaining segmentation types. Overall, the results suggest that, despite some atypical aspects of visuo-spatial perception in WS, the ability to segment a global form into parts is broadly typical in this population. In turn, this informs predictions of brain function in WS, particularly areas V1 and V4.
-
Wilmut K, Wann JP, Brown JH, 'How active gaze informs the hand in sequential pointing movements'
Experimental Brain Research 175 (4) (2006) pp.654-666
ISSN: 0014-4819 eISSN: 1432-1106AbstractPublished hereVisual information is vital for fast and accurate hand movements. It has been demonstrated that allowing free eye movements results in greater accuracy than when the eyes maintain centrally fixed. Three explanations as to why free gaze improves accuracy are: shifting gaze to a target allows visual feedback in guiding the hand to the target (feedback loop), shifting gaze generates ocular-proprioception which can be used to update a movement (feedback-feedforward), or efference copy could be used to direct hand movements (feedforward). In this experiment we used a double-step task and manipulated the utility of ocular-proprioceptive feedback from eye to head position by removing the second target during the saccade. We confirm the advantage of free gaze for sequential movements with a double-step pointing task and document eye-hand lead times of approximately 200 ms for both initial movements and secondary movements. The observation that participants move gaze well ahead of the current hand target dismisses foveal feedback as a major contribution. We argue for a feedforward model based on eye movement efference as the major factor in enabling accurate hand movements. The results with the double-step target task also suggest the need for some buffering of efference and ocular-proprioceptive signals to cope with the situation where the eye has moved to a location ahead of the current target for the hand movement. We estimate that this buffer period may range between 120 and 200 ms without significant impact on hand movement accuracy.
-
Wilmut K, Wann JP, Brown J, 'Problems in the coupling of eye and hand in the sequential movements of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder'
Child: Care, Health and Development 32 (6) (2006) pp.665-678
ISSN: 0305-1862 eISSN: 1365-2214AbstractPublished hereBackground Shifting gaze and attention ahead of the hand is a natural component in the performance of skilled manual actions. Very few studies have examined the precise co-ordination between the eye and hand in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD).
Methods This study directly assessed the maturity of eye–hand co-ordination in children with DCD. A double-step pointing task was used to investigate the coupling of the eye and hand in 7-year-old children with and without DCD. Sequential targets were presented on a computer screen, and eye and hand movements were recorded simultaneously.
Results There were no differences between typically developing (TD) and DCD groups when completing fast single-target tasks. There were very few differences in the completion of the first movement in the double-step tasks, but differences did occur during the second sequential movement. One factor appeared to be the propensity for the DCD children to delay their hand movement until some period after the eye had landed on the target. This resulted in a marked increase in eye–hand lead during the second movement, disrupting the close coupling and leading to a slower and less accurate hand movement among children with DCD.
Conclusions In contrast to skilled adults, both groups of children preferred to foveate the target prior to initiating a hand movement if time allowed. The TD children, however, were more able to reduce this foveation period and shift towards a feedforward mode of control for hand movements. The children with DCD persevered with a look-then-move strategy, which led to an increase in error. For the group of DCD children in this study, there was no evidence of a problem in speed or accuracy of simple movements, but there was a difficulty in concatenating the sequential shifts of gaze and hand required for the completion of everyday tasks or typical assessment items.
Book chapters
-
Wilmut K, Barnett A, 'Movement Coordination, Control & Skill Acquisition in DCD' in Barnett A, Hill E (ed.), Understanding Motor Behaviour in DCD, Routledge (2019)
ISBN: 9781138287501 eISBN: 9781315268231AbstractPublished hereDCD is commonly described as a motor learning disorder. However, surprisingly few studies have specifically examined motor skill learning in individuals with DCD. This chapter will begin by describing studies which have considered skill acquisition in a DCD population. The findings of these studies will then be discussed in light of the theoretical framework put forward in the previous chapter by Newell and Pacheco. Furthermore, we will address issues surrounding motor control in this population including the lack of research focusing on coordination in Developmental Coordination Disorder and we will discuss the issue of variability of movement within a DCD context.
Professional information
Memberships of professional bodies
- Chair of the UK working group for the International Society of Research into DCD
- Member of the ESRC peer review college
- Reviewer for: EPSRC, MRC, ESRC, Experimental Brain Research, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Human Movement Science, Research into Developmental Disabilities, Gait and Posture, Disability and Rehabilitation, Behavioural Brain Research, JEP: HPP.